Preview only show first 10 pages with watermark. For full document please download
Transcript
Dog Training Encyclopedia SmallDogsUSA.org created a compilation of the most important information on dog training available. The Dog Training Encyclopedia is comprised of 12 Categories and 39 Articles with references and resources that expand to complete the most thorough book on dog training available. The Media section is available as a separate publication. Whether dog training is your profession or you just want to decode your dog's behavior, you will discover what you need to know. Free publications, PUP-E-CARDS®, daily news, videos and resources available at smalldogsusa.org or news.smalldogsusa.org His Master's Voice RECOMMENDED: Not only are these training treats used by pros and are extremely effective, but they come with a money-back guarantee and customer support. NOTE: Due to quality control standards, these training treats are only available direct from the manufacturer and not available in stores. Contents 1 CATEGORIES 1 1.1 Dog training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 1.1.1 Definition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 1.1.2 History . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 1.1.3 How dogs learn . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 1.1.4 Training methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 1.1.5 Factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 1.1.6 Individualized and/or class training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 1.1.7 Specialized training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 1.1.8 Tools . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 1.1.9 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 1.1.10 Notes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9 1.1.11 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11 1.1.12 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11 Assistance dog . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11 1.2.1 Classification . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12 1.2.2 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12 1.2.3 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12 1.2.4 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12 Clicker training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13 1.3.1 History . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13 1.3.2 Methodology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13 1.3.3 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14 1.3.4 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14 1.3.5 Further reading . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14 1.3.6 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14 Dog agility . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14 1.4.1 Competition basics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15 1.4.2 Agility obstacles 15 1.4.3 Agility scoring and clean runs 1.4.4 Competition classes 1.4.5 1.2 1.3 1.4 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17 Fairness among dogs and handlers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18 i ii CONTENTS 1.4.6 History . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19 1.4.7 Geography . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19 1.4.8 Training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19 1.4.9 Competition process . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 20 1.4.10 Injuries . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22 1.4.11 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22 1.4.12 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22 1.4.13 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23 Herding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23 1.5.1 Notes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23 1.5.2 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23 Hunting dog . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24 1.6.1 Breeds and capabilities used in hunting . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24 1.6.2 Gallery . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25 1.6.3 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25 1.6.4 Further reading . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25 Leash . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26 1.7.1 Types of leashes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26 1.7.2 Leash laws in the United States of America . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27 1.7.3 Dog leashes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28 1.7.4 Cat leashes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28 1.7.5 Other uses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 1.7.6 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 1.7.7 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 List of dog sports . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 1.8.1 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31 1.8.2 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31 Obedience training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31 1.9.1 History . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31 1.9.2 Dog intelligence and training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32 1.9.3 Commands . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32 1.9.4 Training devices . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33 1.9.5 Competitive obedience . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34 1.9.6 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 1.9.7 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 1.9.8 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 1.10 Operant conditioning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 1.10.1 Historical notes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 1.10.2 Tools and procedures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36 1.10.3 Factors that alter the effectiveness of consequences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38 1.10.4 Operant variability . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39 1.5 1.6 1.7 1.8 1.9 CONTENTS 2 iii 1.10.5 Avoidance learning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39 1.10.6 Four term contingency . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39 1.10.7 Operant hoarding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40 1.10.8 Biological correlates of operant conditioning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40 1.10.9 Operant conditioning in economics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40 1.10.10 Questions about the law of effect . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40 1.10.11 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40 1.10.12 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 41 1.10.13 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 42 1.11 Punishment (psychology) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 42 1.11.1 Types . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 42 1.11.2 Aversives . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43 1.11.3 Importance of contingency and contiguity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43 1.11.4 Applied behavior analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43 1.11.5 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43 1.11.6 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43 1.12 Reinforcement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44 1.12.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44 1.12.2 Brief history . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44 1.12.3 Operant conditioning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45 1.12.4 Natural and artificial . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47 1.12.5 Intermittent reinforcement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47 1.12.6 Schedules . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47 1.12.7 Shaping . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51 1.12.8 Chaining . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51 1.12.9 Persuasive communication & the reinforcement theory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52 1.12.10 Mathematical models . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52 1.12.11 Criticisms . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52 1.12.12 Applications . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 53 1.12.13 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 53 1.12.14 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 53 1.12.15 Further reading . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 54 1.12.16 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 55 ARTICLES 56 2.1 Alpha roll . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56 2.1.1 History . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56 2.1.2 Effects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56 2.1.3 Contemporary use . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56 2.1.4 Further sources . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56 2.1.5 Footnotes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 57 Animal attacks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 57 2.2 iv CONTENTS 2.3 2.2.1 Alligators . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 57 2.2.2 Arthropods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 57 2.2.3 Bears . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 58 2.2.4 Beavers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 58 2.2.5 Birds . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 58 2.2.6 Bulls . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59 2.2.7 Chimpanzees . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59 2.2.8 Catfish . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59 2.2.9 Cats . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59 2.2.10 Cougars . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59 2.2.11 Coyotes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59 2.2.12 Crocodiles . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59 2.2.13 Dogs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59 2.2.14 Elephants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 2.2.15 Fox . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 2.2.16 Hippopotamus . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 2.2.17 Horses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 2.2.18 Hyenas . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 2.2.19 Komodo dragons . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 2.2.20 Leopards . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 2.2.21 Lions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 2.2.22 Racoon . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 2.2.23 Rats . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 2.2.24 Sharks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 2.2.25 Snakes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 2.2.26 Tigers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 61 2.2.27 Wolves . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 61 2.2.28 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 61 2.2.29 Notes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 61 2.2.30 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 61 2.2.31 Bibliography . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65 2.2.32 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65 Dog bite . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 66 2.3.1 Health effects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 66 2.3.2 Causes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 66 2.3.3 Breed-specific attacks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68 2.3.4 Human-dog interaction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68 2.3.5 Legal issues . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69 2.3.6 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69 2.3.7 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69 2.3.8 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 70 CONTENTS v 2.4 Bark (sound) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 70 2.4.1 Barking in dogs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 70 2.4.2 Types of barks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 70 2.4.3 Barking as nuisance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71 2.4.4 Representation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72 2.4.5 Breeds . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73 2.4.6 Naturally “barkless”dog breeds . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73 2.4.7 Barking in other animals . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73 2.4.8 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 74 2.4.9 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 74 2.4.10 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75 Bite inhibition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75 2.5.1 Evolution of Bite Inhibition in Modern Dogs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75 2.5.2 Chemicals Involved in Aggression . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75 2.5.3 Training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75 2.5.4 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76 BowLingual . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76 2.6.1 Features . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76 2.6.2 Versions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76 2.6.3 Effectiveness . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77 2.6.4 Related products . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77 2.6.5 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77 2.6.6 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77 Canine Good Citizen . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77 2.7.1 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78 Dog collar . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78 2.8.1 Basic collars . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78 2.8.2 Training collars . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79 2.8.3 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81 2.8.4 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81 2.8.5 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82 Dog communication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82 2.9.1 Dog-human communication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82 2.9.2 Evolution of dog-human communication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83 2.9.3 Dominance and submission . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83 2.9.4 Visual communication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83 2.9.5 Auditory communication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85 2.9.6 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86 2.9.7 Bibliography . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86 2.9.8 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86 2.9.9 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 2.5 2.6 2.7 2.8 2.9 vi CONTENTS 2.10 Coprophagia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 2.10.1 Humans . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 2.10.2 Other animals . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 2.10.3 Plants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88 2.10.4 History . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88 2.10.5 Society and culture . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88 2.10.6 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88 2.10.7 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88 2.10.8 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89 2.11 Crate training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89 2.11.1 Rationale . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89 2.11.2 Crate selection . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89 2.11.3 Training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 90 2.11.4 Adverse effects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 90 2.11.5 Legislation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91 2.11.6 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91 2.12 Cyber-Enhanced Working Dog . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91 2.12.1 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91 2.12.2 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91 2.13 Defence Animal Centre . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92 2.13.1 History . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92 2.13.2 Function . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92 2.13.3 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92 2.13.4 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92 2.14 Detection dog . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93 2.14.1 Functions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93 2.14.2 Criticism . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 94 2.14.3 Bed bug detection dogs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95 2.14.4 Wildlife scat detection . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95 2.14.5 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95 2.14.6 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 96 2.15 Dog aggression . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97 2.15.1 Factors contributing to aggression . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97 2.15.2 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98 2.15.3 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98 2.15.4 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98 2.16 Dog behavior . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98 2.16.1 Evolution/Domestication/Co-evolution with humans . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98 2.16.2 Cognition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 99 2.16.3 Senses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 99 2.16.4 Social behavior . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 100 CONTENTS vii 2.16.5 Reproduction behavior . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102 2.16.6 Behavior problems . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102 2.16.7 Comparison of behavior with other canids . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102 2.16.8 Dogs in human society . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102 2.16.9 Attacks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102 2.16.10 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103 2.16.11 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103 2.16.12 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 105 2.17 Dog behaviourist . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 105 2.17.1 Overview . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 105 2.17.2 Discipline . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 105 2.17.3 Professional associations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 106 2.17.4 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 106 2.17.5 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 106 2.17.6 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 106 2.18 Dog bite tug . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 106 2.18.1 Materials . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 106 2.18.2 Size and design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 107 2.18.3 Puppy training with a bite tug . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 107 2.18.4 Adult dog training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 107 2.18.5 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 107 2.18.6 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 107 2.18.7 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 107 2.19 Dog surfing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 107 2.19.1 Overview . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 108 2.19.2 History . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 108 2.19.3 Competitions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 108 2.19.4 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 108 2.19.5 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 108 2.19.6 Bibliography . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 109 2.19.7 Further reading . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 109 2.19.8 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 109 2.20 Dog–cat relationship . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 109 2.20.1 Range of relationships . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110 2.20.2 Cultural impact . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110 2.20.3 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110 2.20.4 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110 2.21 Dogs in the City . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 111 2.21.1 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 111 2.21.2 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 111 2.22 Dogs in warfare . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 111 viii CONTENTS 2.22.1 History . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 111 2.22.2 Timeline . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 112 2.22.3 Roles . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 113 2.22.4 Images . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 117 2.22.5 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 117 2.22.6 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 118 2.22.7 Further reading . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 119 2.22.8 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 119 2.23 Fetch (game) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120 2.23.1 Mathematics of Fetch . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120 2.23.2 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120 2.23.3 Further reading . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120 2.23.4 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120 2.24 Flirt pole . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120 2.24.1 Operation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 121 2.24.2 Uses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 122 2.24.3 Health effects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 122 2.24.4 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 123 2.24.5 Notes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 123 2.24.6 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 123 2.25 Game (dog) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 123 2.25.1 Dog fighting breeds . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 123 2.25.2 Working terrier breeds . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 123 2.25.3 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 123 2.25.4 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124 2.26 Gee and haw . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124 2.26.1 In popular culture . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124 2.26.2 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124 2.26.3 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124 2.27 Gun-dog training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124 2.27.1 Types of dogs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124 2.27.2 Training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124 2.27.3 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 126 2.28 Housebreaking . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 126 2.28.1 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 126 2.28.2 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 126 2.29 Dog intelligence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 126 2.29.1 Inherited abilities . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 127 2.29.2 Evaluation of intelligence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 127 2.29.3 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 128 2.29.4 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 128 CONTENTS ix 2.30 The Intelligence of Dogs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 128 2.30.1 Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 129 2.30.2 Evaluation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 129 2.30.3 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 131 2.30.4 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 131 2.31 Pack (canine) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 132 2.31.1 Pack behavior in specific species . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 132 2.31.2 Dominance and the alpha wolf . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 133 2.31.3 See also . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 133 2.31.4 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 134 2.32 Prey drive . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 134 2.32.1 Aspects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 134 2.32.2 Benefits . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 134 2.32.3 Degrees . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 135 2.32.4 Balance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 135 2.32.5 Notes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 135 2.33 Professional handler . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 135 2.33.1 Education . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 135 2.33.2 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 135 2.34 Separation anxiety in dogs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 135 2.34.1 Typical behaviors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 136 2.34.2 Causes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 136 2.34.3 Treatment for separation anxiety in dogs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 136 2.34.4 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 137 2.34.5 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 137 2.35 Shepherd's whistle . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 138 2.35.1 History . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 138 2.35.2 Mechanics and material . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 138 2.35.3 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 138 2.35.4 External links . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 138 2.36 Shock collar . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 138 2.36.1 Types of devices . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 138 2.36.2 Frame of reference . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 139 2.36.3 Technical considerations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 139 2.36.4 Scientific studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 140 2.36.5 Criticism . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 143 2.36.6 Praise . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 144 2.36.7 Public control . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 144 2.36.8 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 145 2.37 Soft mouth . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 145 2.37.1 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 146 x CONTENTS 2.38 Twilight bark . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 146 2.38.1 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 146 2.39 Dog whistle . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 146 2.39.1 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 147 3 Text and image sources, contributors, and licenses 148 3.1 Text . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 148 3.2 Images . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 155 3.3 Content license . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 162 Chapter 1 CATEGORIES 1.1 Dog training tioning, where it forms an association between an antecedent and its consequence.* [2] There are a variety of established methods of animals training, each with its adherents and critics. Some of the better known dog training procedures include the Koehler method, clicker training, dominance-based training, negative reinforcement and relationship-based training. The common characteristics of successful methods are knowing the animal's attributes and personality, accurate timing of reinforcement and/or punishment and consistent communication. 1.1.1 Definition Dancer with a Hoop Dog training is the application of behavior analysis which uses the environmental events of antecedents and consequences to modify the behavior of a dog, either for it to assist in specific activities or undertake particular tasks, or for it to participate effectively in contemporary domestic life. While training dogs for specific roles dates back to Roman times at least, the training of dogs to be com- Dog training using positive reinforcement, with the dog exhibiting patible household pets developed with suburbanization in the “down”position the 1950s. A dog learns from every interaction it has with its environment.* [1] This can be through classical conditioning, where it forms an association between two stimuli; non-associative learning, where its behavior is modified through habituation or sensitisation; and operant condi- Dog training is teaching a response to cues or commands, or the performance of actions not necessarily natural to the dog, and also raising a dog accommodated to his environment by modifying natural digging, barking and eliminating behaviors. Dog training is defined as the purpose1 2 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES ful changing of a dog's behavior.* [3] Dog training can be socialisation to the domestic environment, basic obedience training or training for specialized activities including law enforcement, search and rescue, hunting, working with livestock, assistance to people with disabilities, entertainment, dog sports, detection and protecting people or property. 1.1.2 History to train desired behaviors, advocated the use of compulsion and inducements, differentiated between primary and secondary reinforcers, and described shaping behaviors, chaining components of an activity, and the importance of timing rewards and punishments. The book demonstrated an understanding of the principles of operant conditioning almost thirty years before they were formally outlined by B.F. Skinner in The Behavior of Organisms.* [9] While publishers of the 2001 reprint warn that some of the “compulsive inducements”such as the switch, the spiked collar and the forced compliance are unnecessarily harsh for today's pet dogs,* [10] the basic principles of Most's methods are still used in police and military settings.* [11] Although research into how dogs learn and into crossspecies communication has changed the approach to dog training in recent decades, understanding the role of early trainers and scientists contributes to an appreciation of Marian Breland Bailey played a major role in developing empirically validated and humane animal training how particular methods and techniques developed.* [4] methods and in promoting their widespread implementation.* [12] Marian was a graduate student under B.F. Before 1900 Skinner. Her first husband Keller Breland also came to study with Skinner and they collaborated with him, trainIn around 127-116 B.C. a Roman farmer, Marcus Varro, ing pigeons to guide bombs. The Brelands saw the comrecorded advice on raising and training puppies for herd- mercial possibilities of operant training, founding Aniing livestock. His writings indicate that not only was dog mal Behavior Enterprises (ABE). In 1955, they opened training for specific tasks well established, but that the the “I.Q. Zoo”as both a training facility and a showvalue of early training was recognised.* [5] case of trained animals. They were among the first to use In 1848 W. N. Hutchinson published his book Dog Break- trained animals in television commercials, and the first ing: The Most Expeditious, Certain and Easy Method, to train dolphins and whales as entertainment, as well as * Whether Great Excellence or Only Mediocrity Be Required, for the navy. [12] Keller died in 1965, and in 1976 MarWith Odds and Ends for Those Who Love the Dog and ian married Bob Bailey, who had been director of mathe Gun. Primarily concerned with training hunting dogs rine mammal training for the navy. They pioneered the such as pointers and setters, the book advocates a form of use of the clicker as a conditioned reinforcer for training * reward-based training, commenting on men who have“a animals at a distance. [11] ABE went on to train thou* strong arm and a hard heart to punish, but no temper and sands of animals of more than 140 species. [12] Their no head to instruct”and suggesting “Be to his virtues work had significant public exposure through press covever kind. Be to his faults a little blind.”* [6] Stephen erage of ABE-trained animals, bringing the principles of Hammond, a writer for Forest and Stream magazine, ad- behavior analysis and operant conditioning to a wide au* vocated in his 1882 book Practical Training that hunting dience. [13] dogs be praised and rewarded with meat for doing the Konrad Lorenz, an Austrian scientist who is regarded as correct behavior.* [7] developing the foundations of ethological research,* [14] further popularised animal behaviorism with his books, Man Meets Dog and King Solomon's Ring.* [15] Lorenz War years stated that there were three essential commands to teach a dog: “lie down”(stay where you are), “basket”(go Konrad Most began training dogs for police work in Ger- over there) and “heel”(come with me).* [16] many, and was appointed principal of the State Breeding and Training Establishment for police dogs in Berlin, In 1935, the American Kennel Club began obedience triwhere he carried out original research into training dogs als, and in the following years popular magazines raised for a broad range of service tasks. At the outbreak of public awareness of the benefits of having a trained pet war in 1914 he was charged with organising and direct- dog, and of*the recreational possibilities of dog training ing the use of dogs to further the war effort. He headed as a hobby. [17] After WWII, the increasing complexithe Experimental Institute for Armed Forces' Dogs dur- ties of suburban living demanded that for a pet dog's own ing the Second World War, and afterwards ran the Ger- protection and its owner's convenience, the dog should be man Dog Farm, a centre for the training of working obedient. William Koehler had served as principal trainer dogs, including assistance dogs for the blind. He played a at the War Dog Training Center, in California, and afleading role in the formation of the German Canine Re- ter the war became chief trainer for the Orange Empire search Society and Society for Animal Psychology.* [8] Dog Club̶at the time, the largest dog club in the United clubs, and a dog His 1910 publication, Training Dogs: A Manual, empha- States̶instructor for a number of breed * trainer for the Walt Disney Studios. [18] In 1962 Koehler sised using instinctive behavior such as the prey drive 1.1. DOG TRAINING published The Koehler Method of Dog Training, in which he is highly critical of what he calls“tid-bit training techniques”based in“the prattle of 'dog psychologists'".* [17] Amongst the training innovations attributed to Koehler is the use of a long line in conjunction with a complete absence of oral communication as a way of instilling attentiveness prior to any leash training. Koehler insisted that participants in his training classes used “emphatic corrections”, including leash jerks and throw chains, explaining that tentative, nagging corrections were cruel in that they caused emotional disturbance to the dog.* [19] Vicki Hearne, a disciple of Koehler's, commented on the widespread criticism of his corrections, with the explanation that it was the emotionally loaded language used in the book that led to a number of court cases, and to the book being banned in Arizona for a time.* [20] Despite the controversy, his basic method forms the core of many contemporary training systems.* [21] Post WWII 3 it was acceptable to use “a tidbit now and then to overcome a problem.”Saunders perhaps began the shift away from military and police training methods, stressing repeatedly the importance of reinforcement for good behaviour in training̶a move toward the positive training methods used today.* [23] In 1965, John Paul Scott and John Fuller identified the critical periods for learning and social development in puppies, and published Genetics and the Social Behavior of the Dog, a landmark study of dog behavior.* [24] The 1980 television series Training Dogs the Woodhouse Way made Barbara Woodhouse a household name in the UK, and the first international celebrity dog trainer.* [25] Known for her “no bad dogs”philosophy, Woodhouse was highly critical of “bad owners”, particularly those she saw as“overly sentimental”.* [26] She described the “psychoanalyzing of dogs”as “a lot of rubbish”.* [27] Her no-nonsense style made her a pop-culture icon, with her emphatic“sit”and catch cry of“walkies”becoming part of the popular vernacular.* [28] The Monks of New Skete, who were breeders and trainers of German Shepherds in Cambridge, New York, published How to Be Your Dog's Best Friend: A Training Manual for Dog Owners in 1978 and it became an immediate best seller. Despite advocating a philosophy that “understanding is the key to communication and compassion with your dog,”* [29] they endorsed confrontational punishments which were later shown to elicit dangerously aggressive responses in many dogs.* [30] In the 1980s veterinarian and animal behaviourist Ian Dunbar discovered that despite evidence on the peak learning periods in animals, few dog trainers worked with puppies before they were six months old.* [25] Dunbar founded Sirius Dog Training, the first off-leash training program specifically for puppies, which emphasizes the importance of teaching bite inhibition, sociality, and other basic household manners, to dogs under six months of age.* [31] Dunbar has written numerous books, and is known for his international seminar presentations and award-winning videos on puppy and dog behavior and training.* [32] Rudd Weatherwax trains Lassie. In the 1950s Blanche Saunders was a staunch advocate of pet-dog training, travelling throughout the U.S. to promote obedience classes.* [15] In The Complete Book of Dog Obedience, she said,“Dogs learn by associating their act with a pleasing or displeasing result. They must be disciplined when they do wrong, but they must also be rewarded when they do right.”* [22] Negative reinforcement procedures played a key part in Saunders' method, primarily the jerking of the choke chain. The mantra taught to students was “Command! Jerk! Praise!" She felt that food should not be an ongoing reward, but that Prior to the 1980s, Karen Pryor was a marine-mammal trainer who used Skinner's operant principles to teach dolphins and develop marine-mammal shows. In 1984, she published her book, Don't Shoot the Dog: The New Art of Teaching and Training, an explanation of operantconditioning procedures written for the general public.* [23] In the book Pryor explains why punishment as a way to get people to change often fails, and describes specific positive methods for changing the behaviour of husbands, children and pets.* [33] Pryor's dog training materials and seminars showed how operant procedures can be used to provide training based on positive reinforcement of good behavior.* [23] Pryor and Gary Wilkes introduced clicker training to dog trainers with a series of seminars in 1992 and 1993. Wilkes used aversives as well 4 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES as rewards, and the philosophical differences soon ended Typical positive reinforcement events will satisfy some the partnership.* [34] physiological or psychological need, so it can be food, a game, or a demonstration of affection. Different dogs will find different things reinforcing. Negative reinforcement 21st century occurs when a dog discovers that a particular response ends the presentation of an aversive stimulus. An aversive The 21st century has seen the proliferation of television is anything that the dog does not like, such as a tight choke programs and accompanying books that feature dog train- chain.* [39] ing and rehabilitation,* [35] including Joel Silverman's Good Dog U, Dog Whisperer with Cesar Millan, It's Me Punishment is operationally defined as an event that lowor the Dog featuring Victoria Stillwell, The Underdog ers the probability of the behavior that it follows. It is * Show, Dogs in the City, and SuperFetch. The Association not“punishment”in the common sense of the word, [40] of Pet Dog Trainers advises that television programs are and does not mean physical or psychological harm and produced primarily for entertainment, and while all pro- most certainly does not mean abuse. Punishment simgrams will have good and not-so-good points, the viewer ply involves the presentation of an undesired consequence should critically evaluate the information before deciding (positive punishment) when the wrong behavior is performed, such as a snap of the leash; or the removal of which training tips to adopt.* [36] a desired consequence (negative punishment) when the wrong behavior is performed, such as the owner eating 1.1.3 How dogs learn the cheese that would have been the reward.* [41] A behavior that has previously been developed will cease if Operant conditioning reinforcement stops. This is called extinction. A dog that paws its owner for attention will eventually stop if it no longer receives attention.* [42] Classical conditioning Classical conditioning (or Pavlovian conditioning) is a form of learning in which one stimulus, the conditioned stimulus, comes to signal the occurrence of a second stimulus, the unconditioned stimulus.* [43] Basically, classical conditioning is when a dog learns to associate things in its environment, or discovers some things just go together. A dog may become afraid of rain through an association with thunder and lightning, or it may respond to the owner putting on a particular pair of shoes by fetching Reinforcement can be a game or toy, such as this tennis ball. its leash.* [44] Classical conditioning is used in dog training to help a dog make specific associations with a particOperant conditioning (or instrumental conditioning) is ular stimulus, particularly in overcoming fear of people a form of learning in which an individual's behavior and situations.* [45] is modified by its consequences. Two complementary motivations drive instrumental learning: the maximization of positive outcomes and minimization of aversive Non-associative learning ones.* [37] There are two ways in which behavior is reinforced or strengthened: Positive Reinforcement occurs Non-associative learning is a change in a response to a when a behavior is strengthened by producing some de- stimulus that does not involve associating the presented sirable consequence; negative reinforcement occurs when stimulus with another stimulus or event such as reward a behavior is strengthened by avoiding some undesirable or punishment.* [46] Habituation is non-associative learnconsequence. There are two ways in which behavior is de- ing. An example is where a dog that reacts excitedly to creased or weakened: negative punishment occurs when a door bell is subjected to repeated ringing without aca behavior is weakened by not producing a reinforcing companying visitors, and stops reacting to the meaningconsequence; and positive punishment occurs when a be- less stimuli. It becomes habituated to the noise.* [47] On havior is weakened by producing a consequence that is the other side of habituation, is sensitization. Some dogs' a disincentive. In combination, these basic reinforcing reactions to the stimuli become stronger instead of them and punishing contingencies provide four ways for mod- habituating to the repeated stimuli or event.* [48] Desenifying behavior.* [38] Reinforcement increases the rela- sitization is the process of pairing positive experiences tive probability or frequency of the behavior it follows, with an object, person, or situation that causes fear or while Punishment decreases the relative probability or anxiety.* [49] Consistent exposure to the feared object in frequency of the behaviour it follows. conjunction with rewards allows the animal to become 1.1. DOG TRAINING 5 less stressed, thereby becoming desensitized in the pro- the same skills at six months of age than control puppies cess. This type of training can be effective for dogs who the same age who were not previously allowed to watch are fearful of fireworks.* [50] their mothers working.* [54] A 2001 study recorded the Learned irrelevance is where dogs that are over-exposed behaviour of dogs in detour tests, in which a favourite to a stimulus or cue learn the cue is irrelevant because toy or food was placed behind a V-shaped fence. The the exposure has proven to be uneventful. So a dog demonstration of the detour by humans significantly imowner who continually says “Sit, sit”without response proved the dogs' performance in the trials. The experior consequence, inadvertently teaches the dog to ignore ments showed that dogs are able to rely on information provided by human action when confronted with a new the cue.* [42] task. Significantly, they did not copy the exact path of the Learned helplessness is where a dog just simply shuts human demonstrator, but adopted the detour behaviour down, in a situation where it has no option to avoid a shown by humans to reach their goal.* [55] A 1977 experinegative event. For learned helplessness to occur, the ment by Adler and Adler found that puppies who watched event must be both traumatic and outside the dog's con- other puppies learn to pull a food cart into their cages by trol.* [51] Family dogs that are exposed to unpredictable an attached ribbon proved considerably faster at the task or uncontrolled punishment are at risk of developing dis- when later given the opportunity themselves. At 38 days turbances associated with the learned helplessness disor- of age, the demonstrator puppies took an average of 697 der. Punishment which is poorly coordinated with iden- seconds to succeed, while the observers succeeded in an tifiable avoidance cues or response options, such as when average of 9 seconds.* [56] punishment takes place long after the event, meet the criteria of inescapable trauma.* [41] 1.1.4 Training methods Social learning Social learning is the learning that occurs through observing the behavior of others. This form of learning does not need reinforcement to occur; instead, a model is required. While the model may not be intentionally trying to instill any particular behavior, many behaviors that are observed are remembered and imitated.* [52] The domestic dog is a social species and its social dependency makes it aware of the behavior of others, which contributes to his own behavior and learning abilities. There is, however, ongoing discussion about how much, and how, dogs can learn by interacting with each other and with people.* [53] The term “social learning”encompasses several closely related concepts: allelomimetic behavior or mimicking where, for example, puppies follow or copy others of their kind; social facilitation where the presence of another dog causes an increase in the intensity of a behaviour; and local enhancement which includes pieces of social facilitation, mimicking, and trial-and-error learning, but is different from true observational learning in that the dog actively participates in the behavior in the presence of the other dog and/or other environmental cues.* [53] Four necessary conditions for observational learning are: attention, retention, motivation, and production. That is, the dog must pay attention to the dog or person performing the modelled behavior; retain the information gathered about the behavior during the observation; be motivated to reproduce the behavior in a time and place removed from the original; and finally, produce the behavior, or some reasonable facsimile thereof.* [53] Koehler method Strictly following the model set out in the Koehler Method of Dog Training, some 50 years later, the Koehler method continues to be taught in both class and private training formats. The method is based in the philosophy that a dog acts on its right to choose its actions. Koehler explained that a dog's learned behavior is an act of choice based on its own learning experience. When those choices are influenced by the expectation of reward, the behavior will most likely be repeated, and when those choices are influenced by the anticipation of punishment, they will most likely cease. Once the dog has learned that its choices result in comfort or discomfort it can be taught to make the correct decisions. Action→Memory→Desire encapsulates the learning pattern used by the method; the dog acts, remembers the consequences, and forms the desire to repeat or avoid those consequences. Adherents believe that once the behavior has been correctly taught, it should be performed, thus making any correction, fair, reasonable, and expected.* [57] While the model has been used consistently since 1962, some of the punishment procedures described in the book are now not considered necessary, humane, or appropriate by many trainers.* [23] Motivational training Purely positive or motivational training employs the use of rewards to reinforce good behavior, and ignores all bad behavior.* [58] It is based in Thorndike's Law of Effect, A 1997 study conducted by Slabbert and Rasa deter- which says that actions that produce rewards tend to inthat do not produce remined that pups between the ages of 9–12 weeks who crease in frequency and actions * wards decrease in frequency. [59] were permitted to observe their narcotics-detecting mothers at work generally proved more capable at learning Motivational training has its roots in captive animal train- 6 ing, where compulsion and corrections are both difficult and dangerous, and ignoring bad behavior is not problematic as the animal lives under controlled conditions. As a dog training strategy, purely positive training is feasible, but difficult, as it requires time and patience to control the rewards the dog receives for behavior. Some activities such as jumping up or chasing squirrels are intrinsically rewarding, the activity is its own reward, and with some activities the environment may provide reinforcement such as when the response from dog next door encourages barking.* [58] CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES sit, for example; or by 'shaping' where increasingly closer approximations to the desired behaviour are reinforced; and by 'capturing' where the dog's spontaneous offering of the behaviour is rewarded.* [63] Once a behaviour is learnt and is on cue (command), the clicker and the treats are faded out.* [64] Clicker training uses no physical compulsion or corrections and uses almost entirely positive reinforcements. Some clicker trainers use mild corrections such as a“non reward marker"; an “Uhuh”or “Whoops”to let the dog know that the behaviour is not correct, or corrections Ruff Love is one program based on the method. Stating such as a “Time out”where attention is removed from that “positive is not permissive”the program controls the dog.* [65] the dog's environment using crates, tethers, and head halters to ensure the dog has little opportunity for bad behaviour and to ensure that the owner delivers all reinforcements.* [60] Electronic training Clicker training Electronic training involves the use of an electric shock as an aversive. Common forms are collars which can be triggered remotely, or that are triggered by barking, fencing that delivers a shock when a dog wearing a special collar crosses a buried wire, and mats that can be placed on furniture to deliver a shock. Some aids deliver an aversive such as a spray of citronella when triggered.* [66] The use of electric shock aversives for training dogs is the subject of considerable controversy. Supporters claim that the use of electronic devices allows training at a distance and the potential to eliminate self-rewarding behaviour, and point out that properly used, they have less risk of stress and injury than mechanical devices, such as choke chains. Opponents cite the risks of physical and psychological trauma associated with incorrect or abusive use.* [67] Clicker-training using a metal cricket Clicker training is a nickname given to a positive reinforcement training system based on operant conditioning. The system uses conditioned reinforcers which are able to be delivered more quickly and more precisely than primary reinforcers such as food. The term 'clicker' comes from a small metal cricket adapted from a child's toy, however some trainers using the method use a whistle, a word, or even a light as the conditioned reinforcer.* [61] The basis of effective clicker training is precise timing to deliver the conditioned reinforcer at the same moment as the desired behaviour is offered. The clicker is used as a 'bridge' between the marking of the behaviour and the rewarding with a primary reinforcer such as a treat or a toy.* [62] The behaviour can be elicited by 'luring' where a hand gesture or a treat is used to coax the dog to In one study laboratory-bred Beagles were divided into three groups. Group A received an electric shock when the dogs touched the prey (a rabbit dummy fixed to a motion device). Group H received a shock when they did not obey a previously trained recall command during hunting. Dogs in group R received the electric shock arbitrarily, i.e. the shock was administered unpredictably and out of context. Group A did not show a significant rise in salivary cortisol levels, while group R and group H did show a significant rise. This led to the conclusion that animals which were able to clearly associate the electric stimulus with their action, i.e. touching the prey, and consequently were able to predict and control the stressor, did not show considerable or persistent stress indicators, while animals that were not able to control the situation to avoid the shock did show significant stress.* [67] In 2004 a study was published on German Shepherds trained for protection work using shock collars, which showed that although electronically trained dogs can excel as guard dogs, their behavior toward humans and work circumstances changed, often indicating heightened uncertainty and reactivity.* [68] 1.1. DOG TRAINING Model-rival training Based on the principles of social learning, model-rival training uses a model, or a rival for attention, to demonstrate the desired behaviour.* [69] The method was used by Irene Pepperberg to train Alex the African Grey Parrot to label a large number of objects. McKinley and Young undertook a pilot study on the applicability of a modified version of the model-rival method to the training of domestic dogs, noting that the dog's origins as a member of large and complex social groups promote observational learning. The model-rival training involved an interaction between the trainer, the dog, and a person acting as a model-rival, that is, a model for desired behaviour and a rival for the trainer's attention. In view of the dog, a dialogue concerning a particular toy commenced between the trainer and the model-rival. The trainer praised or scolded the model-rival depending on whether the modelrival had named the toy correctly. It was found that the performance times for completion of the task were similar for dogs trained with either operant conditioning or the model rival method. In addition, the total training time required for task completion was comparable for both methods.* [70] A Hungarian dog training group called Népszigeti Kutyaiskola use a variation of model-rival training which they describe as the Mirror Method. The mirror method philosophy is that dogs instinctively learn by following the example of others in their social sphere. Core to the program is including the dog in all aspects of the owner's life and positive reinforcement of copying behaviors. Mirror method dog training relies on using a dog's natural instincts and inclinations rather than working against them.* [71] Dominance-based training The concepts of“pack”and“dominance”in relation to dog training originated in the 1940s and were popularized by the Monks of New Skete in the 1970s. The model is based on a theory that“dogs are wolves”and since wolves live in hierarchical packs where an alpha male rules over everyone else, then humans must dominate dogs in order to modify their behavior.* [72] However, recent studies have shown that wolves in the wild actually live in nuclear families where the father and mother are considered the pack leaders, and their offspring's status depends on their birth order which does not involve fighting to attain a higher rank, because the young wolves naturally follow their parents' lead.* [73] 7 afraid.* [74] Researchers have described several reasons why the dominance model is a poor choice for dog training.* [75] First, a relationship based on dominance is established to gain priority access to scarce resources, not to impose particular behaviors on the less dominant animal,* [76] so the dominance model is irrelevant for most of the behaviors that people want from their dogs, such as coming when called or walking calmly on a leash.* [75] Second dominance-submission relationships, once established, are constantly tested and must be regularly reinforced.* [77] Thus people, particularly children and the elderly, may not be able to retain their rank and are at risk of being injured if they attempt to do so.* [75] Third, dominant individuals gain priority access to resources, but only while they are present, establishing dominance over a dog does not guarantee its behavior when the dominant individual is distant or absent.* [75] Relationship-based training Derived from the theories of symbolic interactionism, relationship based training exploits the patterns of communication, interpretation and adjustment between dogs and their trainers. Building on a positive relationship between them, the method sets out to achieve results that benefit both the dog and the trainer, while at the same time enhancing and strengthening their relationship. The basic principles include ensuring that the dog's basic needs have been met before beginning a training session, finding out what motivates the dog and using it to elicit behaviours, interpreting the dog's body language to improve communication between dog and trainer, using positive reinforcement to encourage desired behavior, training incompatible behaviors to replace unwanted behaviours, and controlling the dog's environment to limit the possibility of unwanted behaviours.* [78] A relationship-based approach to dog training is not reliant on using particular training aids or treats, the relationship is always there, and the connection between dog and trainer is sufficiently powerful to achieve the training goals.* [79] 1.1.5 Factors Training can take as many forms as there are trainers, however a detailed study of animal trainers found common characteristics of successful methods: thoughtful interpretation of what the animal does prior to training, accurate timing and consistent communication.* [80] Animal behaviorists assert that using dominance to modify a behavior can suppress the behavior without addressCommunication ing the underlying cause of the problem. It can exacerbate the problem and increase the dog's fear, anxiety, and aggression. Dogs that are subjected to repeated threats Main article: Dog communication may react with aggression not because they are trying to be dominant, but because they feel threatened and Dogs have become closely associated with humans 8 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES through domestication and have also become sensitive to human communicative signals. Generally, they have a lot of exposure to human speech, especially during play, and are believed to have a good ability to recognize human speech. Two studies investigated the ability of a single dog that was believed to be exceptional in its understanding of language. Both studies revealed the potential for at least some dogs to develop an understanding of a large number of simple commands on the basis of just the sounds emitted by their owners. However the studies suggested that visual cues from the owner may be important for the understanding of more complex spoken commands.* [81] Understanding Main articles: Dog behavior and Dog intelligence For any of these techniques, consistency of the owner's training/behavior and level of engagement can influence the affectiveness of any technique applied.* [82] Innate characteristics Class training can be effective in encouraging socialization and play with a peer group. Popular advocates of class training include Victoria Stillwell* [86] and Ian Dunbar.* [87] 1.1.7 Specialized training Dogs are also trained for specific activities such as Competitive Obedience, CGC Certification, Agility, Herding, Tracking, and Flyball, and to undertake particular roles such as Detection dogs, Assistance dogs, Hunting dogs, Police dogs, Search and rescue dogs or Guard dogs. Arthur Haggerty, who for forty years was the major supplier of trained dogs for the U.S. entertainment industry, advocated the teaching of tricks to pet dogs, explaining that dogs bred for active duty herding, guarding or hunting were unemployed in modern society. He believed that dogs that are bored or frustrated, and consequently badly behaved, would find a purpose, a stronger relationship with their owners, and a way of filling their idle hours in learning tricks.* [88] Haggerty advocated working with the breed or the individual dog's characteristics to teach tricks based on retrieving, scenting, vocalising and so on, publishing a trick aptitude chart for various dog breeds. He distinguished between tricks based on the dog's normal behaviours (Kiss, Wag your Tail) and tricks that were taught. While Haggerty was publicly critical of trainers using total positive reinforcement for obedience training,* [89] he encouraged food rewards for trick training.* [88] In considering the natural behaviours of specific breeds of dogs, it is possible to train them to perform specialised, highly useful, tasks. For example, Labrador retrievers are the favoured breed for the detection of explosives. This is because of a combination of factors including their food drive which enables them to keep focused on a task despite noise and other distractions. Most working breeds 1.1.8 Tools of dogs are able to be trained to find people with their sense of smell (as opposed to their sense of sight). Cocker Spaniels are able to be trained as part of a termite detec- 1.1.9 See also tion team. Their relatively small size enables them to fit • Alpha roll into small spaces, and their light weight allows them to walk on areas of ceiling which would be dangerous to • Bark (dog) anything heavier. In fact, although unusual, termite de• Conformation showing tection dogs are much more reliable at detecting termites than humans who rely on a basic system of tapping and • Dog agility listening. Because of their ability to learn signals by sight and for their energetic and athletic natures, German Shep• Dog sports herds are able to be trained for work alongside search and • List of dog trainers rescue teams and human apprehension teams.* [83] • Obedience training 1.1.6 Individualized and/or class training Individualised training is used with dogs that have an urgent or unique training problem such as fear, hyperactivity, aggression (and other related problems), separation anxiety, biting, excessive barking, insecurity, destructive behaviors, walking difficulties, and inappropriate elimination.* [84]* [85] This type of training would normally be undertaken where the problem naturally occurs rather than a class situation. General: • Animal cognition • Animal training • Ethology • Operant conditioning • Punishment (psychology) 1.1. DOG TRAINING • Reinforcement 9 • Reward system [24] Scott, John Paul; John L. Fuller (1998). Genetics and the Social Behavior of the Dog. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 9780226743387. • Dog behaviorist [25] Millan 2010, p. 89. [26] Woodhouse 1982, p. 13. 1.1.10 Notes [1] Millan 2010, p. 33. [2] Braslau-Schneck, Stacy (1998).“An Animal Trainer's Introduction To Operant and Classical Conditioning”. Retrieved 29 November 2012. [3] Millan 2010, p. 32. [4] Burch 1999, p. 1. [5] Millan 2010, p. 82. [6] Hutchinson 2005, p.11. [7] Millan 2010, p. 83. [8] Most 1954, p. 7. [27] Woodhouse 1982, p. 9. [28] Dudman, Helga (1996). The Dog's Guide to Famous Owners: A Walkies Through History with Some Very Important Pooches. London: Robson. [29] Monks of New Skete 1978, p. 3. [30] Herron, Meghan E; Frances S. Shofer; Ilana R. Reisner (2009). “Survey of the use and outcome of confrontational and non-confrontational training methods in client-owned dogs showing undesired behaviors” . Applied Animal Behavior Science 117 (1): 47– 54. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2008.12.011. Retrieved 30 November 2012. [31] Dunbar, Ian. “Sirius Dog Training”. Retrieved 30 November 2012. [9] Burch, Mary R; Duane Pickel (1990). “A toast to [32] Millan 2010, p. 90. Most: Konrad Most, a 1910 pioneer in animal training” . Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 23 (2): 263–4. [33] Pryor 1984, p. ix. doi:10.1901/jaba.1990.23-263. PMC 1286234. PMID [34] Spector, Morgan (3 January 2009).“Who Started Clicker 16795731. Training for Dogs?". Retrieved 30 November 2012. [10] Most 1954, p. 26. [35] Millan 2010, p. 91. [11] Millan 2010, p. 84. [36] APDT (2010). “Can I Train My Dog Just Like They Do On TV? Reality TV versus Real Life”. Association of [12] Bihm, Elson M.; J. Arthur Gillaspy, Jr. (1 June 2012). Pet Dog Trainers. Retrieved 30 November 2012. “Marian Breland Bailey (1920–2001)". The Encyclopedia of Arkansas History & Culture. The Central Arkansas [37] Lindsay 2000, p. 247-248 Library System. Retrieved 30 November 2012. [38] Lindsay 2000, p. 246-247. [13] Bailey, R. E.; J. A. Gillaspy, Jr. (2005). “Operant Psychology Goes to the Fair: Marian and Keller Breland in [39] Lindsay 2000, p. 253. the Popular Press, 1947–1966”. The Behavior Analyst [40] Lindsay 2000, p. 251. 28 (2): 143–159. PMC 2755380. PMID 22478446. [14] Tinbergen, N (1963). “On aims and methods of ethology”. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie 20 (4): 410–433. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1963.tb01161.x. [15] Millan 2010, p. 87. [16] Lorenz 1953, p. 43. [17] Koehler 1962, p. 6. [18] Koehler 1962, p. 7. [19] Koehler 1962, p. 8. [20] Hearne 1987, p. 10. [21] Millan 2010, p. 88. [22] Saunders 1969, p. 11. [23] Burch, Mary R. (1 August 2012). “The Evolution of Modern-Day Dog Training”. National Animal Interest Alliance. Retrieved 30 November 2012. [41] Reid 1996, p. 108. [42] Lindsay 2000, p. 213. [43] Bouton, M. E. (2007). Learning and Behavior: A Contemporary Synthesis. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer. [44] Burch 1999, p. 3–5. [45] Dunbar, Ian (2007).“Classical Conditioning”. Dog Star Daily. Retrieved 1 December 2012. [46] “Animal Learning”. Encyclopedia Britannica. Retrieved September 21, 2011. [47] Reid 1996, p. 34–35. [48] Lindsay 2000, p. 219. [49] “The Use of Positive Reinforcement Training Techniques to Enhance the Care, Management, and Welfare of Primates in the laboratory”. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science. 2003. Retrieved 16 December 2011. 10 [50] “Burning Question: Why do dogs hate fireworks?". shine.yahoo.com/. July 3, 2012. Retrieved July 13, 2012. [51] Seligman, Martin E. P.; Steven F. Maier and James H. Geer (1968).“Alleviation of Learned Helplessness in the Dog”. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 73 (3): 256–262. doi:10.1037/h0025831. PMID 5658526 [52] Bandura, Albert (1971). Psychological Modelling. New York: Lieber-Antherton. [53] Miller, Pat (July 2004). “Young Dogs Can Learn From Older Well-Behaved Dogs”. The Whole Dog Journal. Retrieved 1 December 2012. [54] Slabbert, J. M.; O. A. E. Rasa (1997). “Observational learning of an acquired maternal behaviour pattern by working dog pup: an alternative training method?". Applied Animal Behaviour Science 53 (4): 309–316. doi:10.1016/S0168-1591(96)01163-X. [55] Pongrácz, Péter; Á. Miklósi, E. Kubinyi, K. Gurobi, J. Topál and V. Csanyi (2001). “Social learning in dogs: the effect of a human demonstrator on the performance of dogs (Canis familiaris) in a detour task”. Animal Behaviour 62 (6): 1109–1117. doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1866. [56] Adler, Leonore Loeb; Helmut E. Adler (1977). “Ontogeny of observational learning in the dog (Canis familiaris)". Developmental Psychobiology 10 (3): 267–271. doi:10.1002/dev.420100310. PMID 863122. [57] Ancheta, Tony. “Koehler Dog Training”. Retrieved 2 December 2012. [58] Marlo 1999, p. 101. [59] Burch 1999, p. 4. [60] Garrett, Susan (2002). Ruff Love. Chicopee, MA: Clean Run. ISBN 1-892694-06-9. [61] Pryor 1999, p. 4. [62] Pryor 1999, p. 29. [63] Pryor 1999, p. 60–62. [64] Grobbelaar, Claire. “What is Clicker Training?". Retrieved 2 December 2012. [65] Alexander, Melissa (1 July 2003). ""NRMs”No Reward Markers”. Retrieved 2 December 2012. CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES [69] McConnell, Patricia (26 January 2011). “The ModelRival Method”. The Other End of the Leash. Retrieved 2 December 2012. [70] McKinley, S.; R. J. Young (2003). “The efficacy of the model-rival method when compared with operant conditioning for training domestic dogs to perform a retrievalselection task”. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 81 (4): 357–365. doi:10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00277-0 [71] Wogan, Lisa (November 2010). “The Mirror Method”. The Bark. Retrieved 3 December 2012. [72] “Why Won't Dominance Die?". Association of Pet Behavior Counsellors. Retrieved 16 December 2011. [73] “Dog Training and the Myth of Alpha-Male Dominance” . TIME Science. 30 July 2010. Retrieved 16 December 2011. [74] “Position Statement on the Use of Dominance Theory in Behavior Modification of Animals”. American Veterinary Society of Animal Behavior. Retrieved 16 December 2011. [75] Yin, Sophia (2007).“Dominance versus leadership in dog training”. Compendium on Continuing Education for the Practising Veterinarian, North American Edition 29 (7): 414–4–8 [76] Bernstein, Irwin S. (1981). “Dominance: The Baby and the Bathwater”. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3): 419–429. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00009614 [77] Drews, Carlos (1993). “The concept and definition of dominance in animal behaviour”. Behaviour 125 (3/4): 283–313. doi:10.1163/156853993X00290 [78] Woodard, Sherry (15 April 2011). “Why We Use Relationship-Based Training”. Retrieved 2 December 2012. [79] Clothier, Suzanne (2009).“Relationship Based Approach to Training”. Retrieved 2 December 2012. [80] McGreevy 2011, p. 280. [81] Fukuzawa, M.; D. S. Mills; J. J. Cooper (2005). “The effect of human command phonetic characteristics on auditory cognition in dogs (Canis familiaris)". Journal of Comparative Psychology 119 (1): 117–120. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.119.1.117. PMID 15740436. [82] Arhant, Christine (2010). "Behavior of smaller and larger dogs: Effects of training methods, inconsistency of owner behavior and level of engagement in activities with the [67] Schalke, E.; J. Stichnoth; S. Ott; R. Jones-Baade dog”. Applied Animal Behavior Science 123 (3): 131– (2007). “Clinical signs caused by the use of elec142. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2010.01.003. |first2= misstric training collars on dogs in everyday life situations” ing |last2= in Authors list (help); |first3= missing |last3= . Applied Animal Behaviour Science 105 (4): 369–380. in Authors list (help); |first4= missing |last4= in Authors doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2006.11.002. list (help); |first5= missing |last5= in Authors list (help) [66] Burch 1999, p. 162. [68] Schilder, Matthijs B.H.; Joanne A.M. van der Borg (2004). “Training dogs with help of the shock collar: short and long term behavioral effects”. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 85 (3): 319–334. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2003.10.004. [83] McGreevy 2011, p. 116–279. [84] Jane S. Orihel.“Management and Rehabilitation of InterDog Aggression in Animal Shelters”. Retrieved 15 December 2011. 1.2. ASSISTANCE DOG [85] “Specializing in Dog Aggression and Dog Behavior”. Dog Behavior Rehab. Retrieved 15 December 2011. [86] “Positively”. Victoria Stillwell. Retrieved 15 December 2011. [87] “Sirius Dog Training”. Ian Dunbar. Retrieved 15 December 2011. [88] Haggerty, Capt. Arthur; Carol Lea Benjamin (1978). Dog Tricks: Teaching your Dog to be Useful, Fun and Entertaining. New York: Howell Book House. ISBN 0-38513493-2. [89] Miller, Stephen (July 17, 2006). “Captain Haggerty, 74, Dog Trainer, Dog Author, Dog Cineaste”. The New York Sun. Retrieved 1 December 2012. [90] Victorian Consolidated Regulations (2008). “Prevention of Cruelty to Animals Regulations 2008 - SECT 9”. Retrieved 2 December 2012 [91] Lindsay, 2005, p. 583 [92] Lindsay, 2005, p. 584 11 • Millan, Cesar; and Melissa Jo Peltier (2010). Cesar's Rules, New York: Three Rivers Press ISBN 978-0307716873 • Monks of New Skete (1978). How to be Your Dog's Best Friend: A Training Manual for Dog Owners, London : Little Brown • Most, K. (1954). Training Dogs, (J. Cleugh, Trans.), New York: Dogwise Publishing, 2001. ISBN 1929242-00-X • Pryor, Karen (1984). Don't Shoot the Dog: The New Art of Teaching and Training, New York: Bantam Books. ISBN 0-553-38039-7 • Pryor, Karen (1999). Clicker Training for Dogs, London: Ringpress Books. ISBN 1-86054-282-4 • Reid, Pamela J. (1996). Excel-Erated Learning, Explaining (in Plain English) How Dogs Learn and How Best to Teach Them, James & Kenneth Publishers. [93] “Ogmore illegal shock collar dog owner gets £2,000 fine” . BBC News. 18 July 2011. • Saunders, Blanche (1969). Training You to Train Your Dog, New York: Howell Book House. ISBN 0-876-05457-2 [94] Victorian Consolidated Regulations (2008). “Prevention of Cruelty to Animals Regulations 2008 - SECT 17”. Retrieved 2 December 2012 • Scott, John P.; and John L. Fuller (1965). Genetics and the Social Behavior of the Dog, Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 1.1.11 • Woodhouse, Barbara (1982). No Bad Dogs: the Woodhouse Way, New York, Simon & Schuster. ISBN 0-671-54185-4 References • Burch, Mary R.; and Jon S. Bailey (1999). How Dogs Learn, New York: Howell Book House ISBN 1.1.12 0-8760-5371-1 • Hearne, Vicki (1987). Adam's Task: Calling Animals by Name, New York: Alfred A. Knopf ISBN 0-394-75530-8 • Hutchinson, Lieut-Gen WN (1865). Dog Breaking for the Gun: The Most Expeditious, Certain and Easy Method, With Copious Notes on Shooting Sports, New York: Vintage Dog Books, 2005 ISBN 9-781-84664035-3 External links 1.2 Assistance dog Not to be confused with Service dog or Working dog. An assistance dog is a dog trained to aid or assist a • Lindsay, Steven R. (2000). Handbook of Applied Dog Behavior and Training, Vol 1, Adaptation and Learning, Iowa State Press • Lorenz, Konrad (1953). Man Meets Dog, (Marjorie Kerr Wilson, Trans.) Hagerstown, MA: Kodansha America, 1994 • Marlo, Shelby (1999). New Art of Dog Training, Chicago: Contemporary Books, ISBN 0-8092An assistance dog pressing a button to open an automatic door. 3170-0 • McGreevy, P., and R. Boakes (2011). Carrots and person with a disability. Many are trained by a specific Sticks: Principles of Animal Training, Sydney: Dar- organization, others by their handler, sometimes with the lington Press help of a professional trainer. 12 1.2.1 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES Classification Assistance dogs fall into three general types: Guide, Hearing, and Service.* [1] Most will be trained for only one specialty, though “combination”dogs do exist. • Guide dogs assist the blind and the visually impaired. • Hearing dogs, or signal dogs, help the deaf and hard of hearing. Mobility assistance dog helping his handler stand up. 1.2.2 See also • Assistance animal • Autism service dog • Dogs for the Disabled (in the UK) • Medical response dog • Mobility assistance dog • Psychiatric service dog Assistance Dog at food court at shopping mall. • Service dogs are not specifically trained for visual or hearing impairment, but may have specific roles such as mobility assistance dogs, medical alert dogs, and psychiatric service dogs. • Seizure dog • Therapy dog • Working dog 1.2.3 References In the United States, the term“service dog”may be used synonymously with“assistance dog,”and is occasionally [1] Assistance Dogs International used for other types of working dogs as well. These dogs can in some instances be dual classified as therapy dogs. Also any of the above named dogs“in training”are pro- 1.2.4 External links tected under the Americans with Disabilities Act of 1990 • Service Dogs and More - A large print site about (ADA) and Americans with Disabilities Amendment Act Assistance Dogs (ADAA, which expanded some legal protections), even if the handler at the time is not “using”the dog in the ca• Assistance Dogs International pacity for which it is being trained. In most of the rest of the world a distinct separation between service dogs and • Delta Society's National Service Animal Resource assistance dogs is observed. Center 1.3. CLICKER TRAINING • DMOZ Open Directory Project: Service Dogs • International Association of Assistance Dog Partners • Service Dog Central (includes guide and hearing dogs as well) • Please Don't Pat Me Australia (information about Australian Assistance Dogs including guide, hearing, medical, psychiatric assistance dogs and the relevant laws and minimum training standards required) 1.3 Clicker training 13 Keller Breland, worked with him researching pigeon behavior and training projects during World War II, when pigeons were taught to “bowl”(push a ball with their beaks).* [6] They believed that traditional animal training was being needlessly hindered because methods of praise and reward then in use did not inform the animal of success with enough promptness and precision to create the required cognitive connections for speedy learning. They saw the potential for using the operation conditioning method in commercial animal training.* [7] The two later married and in 1947 created Animal Behavior Enterprises (ABE), “the first commercial animal training business to intentionally and systematically incorporate the principles of behavior analysis and operant conditioning into animal training.”* [8] The Brelands coined the term“bridging stimulus”in the 1940s to refer to the function of a secondary reinforcer such as a whistle or click.* [8] ABE continued operations until 1990, with the assistance of Bob Bailey after Keller Breland died in 1965.* [8] They report having trained over 15,000 animals and over 150 species during their time in operation.* [8] Although the Brelands tried to promote clicker training for dogs in the 1940s and 1950s, the method failed to catch on until the late 1980s and early 1990s.* [9] In the early 1990s, Karen Pryor and Gary Wilkes started giving clicker training seminars to dog owners; the first was given in San Franscisco in 1992.* [9] In 1998, Alexandra Kurland published “Clicker Training For Your Horse.” * [9]* [10] In the 21st century, training books began to appear for other companion animals, such as cats, birds, and rabbits.* [11] 1.3.2 Methodology Clicker-training a dog. Clicker training is a method for training animals that uses positive reinforcement such as a food treat, along with a clicker or small mechanical noisemaker to mark the behavior being reinforced. When training a new behavior, the clicker helps the animal to quickly identify the precise behavior that results in the treat. The technique is popular with dog trainers, but can be used for all kinds of domestic and wild animals.* [1] Sometimes instead of a click to mark the desired behavior, other distinctive sounds are made (such as“whistle, a cluck of the tongue, a snap of the fingers, or even a word” )* [2] or visual or other sensory cues (such as a flashlight, hand sign, or vibrating collar),* [3] especially helpful for deaf animals. 1.3.1 The first step in clicker training is teaching the animal to associate the clicker sound (or other chosen marker such as a whistle)* [2] with a treat. Every time the click sounds, a treat is offered immediately. Next the click is used to signal that a desired behavior has happened. Some approaches* [1] are: • capturing: catching the animal in the act of doing something that is desired, for example sitting or lying down. Eventually the animal learns to repeat the behavior for a treat. • shaping: gradually building a new behavior by rewarding each small steps toward it. • luring: using the treat like a magnet to get the animal to move toward the desired position. History Once the behavior is learned, the final step is to add a cue B. F. Skinner first identified and described the princi- for the behavior, such as a word or a hand signal.* [1] The ples of operant conditioning that are used in clicker train- animal will have learned that a treat is on the way after ing.* [4]* [5] Two students of Skinner's, Marian Kruse and completing the desired behavior. 14 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES 1.3.3 See also • Animal training • Dog training • Operant conditioning • Cat training • The Amazing Acro-Cats 1.3.4 References [1] “Clicker Training Your Pet”, ASPCA, accessed July 28, 2014. [2] “5 Clicker Training Myths”, Wag the Dog, accessed July 29, 2014. [3] “Clicker Training for Deaf Dogs”, Deaf Dog Education Action Fund, accessed July 29, 2014. [4] Skinner, B.F. (1951). How to teach animals. Scientific American, 185, 26-29. [5] Skinner, B.F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-CenturyCrofts. • Orr, Joan and Teresa Lewin, “Getting Started: Clicking With Your Rabbit”(2006, Sunshine Books), ISBN 978-1890948238. • Pryor, Karen“Getting Started: Clicker Training for Cats”(2012, Karen Pryor Clickertraining), ISBN 978-1-890948-14-6 (Kindle edition). • Pryor, Karen,“Getting Started: Clicker Training for Dogs”(2004, Interpret Publishing), ISBN 1-86054282-4 • Pryor, Karen,“Reaching the Animal Mind: Clicker Training and What It Teaches Us About All Animals”(2010, Scribner), ISBN 978-0743297776. • Spector, Morgan,“Clicker Training for Obedience” (1999, Sunshine Books), ISBN 978-0962401787. 1.3.6 External links • “Clicker Training Your Pet”, ASPCA 1.4 Dog agility [6] Peterson, G. (2000). The Discovery of Shaping or B.F. Skinnerʼs Big Surprise. The Clicker Journal: The Magazine for Animal Trainers, 43, 6-13. [7] Bailey and Gillaspy, Operant Conditioning Goes to the Fair,The Behavior Analyst 2005, pp 143-159. [8] “Animal Behavior Enterprises”, History of Behavior Analysis, accessed July 28, 2014. [9] “Modern Training and Clicker Training for Pet Owners” , History of Behavior Analysis, accessed July 28, 2014. [10] Kurland, Alexandra, “Clicker Training for Your Horse” (2004 edition, Ringpress Books), ISBN 1-86054-292-1. A hairless Chinese Crested taking part in an agility competition. [11] See “Further Reading”. Dog agility is a dog sport in which a handler directs a dog through an obstacle course in a race for both time 1.3.5 Further reading and accuracy. Dogs run off leash with no food or toys as incentives, and the handler can touch neither dog nor • Alexander, Melissa C., “Click for Joy: Questions obstacles.* [1]* [2]* [3]* [4]* [5]* [6] Consequently the hanand Answers from Clicker Trainers and Their Dogs” dler's controls are limited to voice, movement, and var(2003, Sunshine Books), ISBN 978-1890948122. ious body signals, requiring exceptional training of the animal and coordination of the handler. • Castro, A. (2007): The bird school - Clicker training for parrots and other birds. ISBN 978-3-939770- In its simplest form, an agility course consists of a set of standard obstacles laid out by a judge in a design of his 03-9. or her own choosing in an area of a specified size. The • Johnson, Melinda,“Getting Started: Clicker Train- surface may be of grass, dirt, rubber, or special matting. ing for Birds”(2003, Sunshine Books), ISBN 978- Depending on the type of competition, the obstacles may 1890948153. be marked with numbers indicating the order in which • Kurland, Alexandra, “Clicker Training for Your they must be completed. Horse”(2004, Ringpress Books), ISBN 1-86054292-1. Courses are complicated enough that a dog could not complete them correctly without human direction. In 1.4. DOG AGILITY 15 competition, the handler must assess the course, decide on handling strategies, and direct the dog through the course, with precision and speed equally important. Many strategies exist to compensate for the inherent difference in human and dog speeds and the strengths and weaknesses of the various dogs and handlers. • Agility field left side: A competition agility field showing (clockwise from lower left) a tunnel, the dogwalk, the judge standing in front of a winged jump, two additional winged jumps, dog executing the teeter-totter with his handler guiding, and the tire jump. 1.4.1 • Course map showing the layout of the course in the preceding photos. Maps like this are commonly used by officials to communicate the course to handlers. Competition basics Because each course is different, handlers are allowed a short walk-through before the competition starts. During this time, all handlers competing in a particular class can walk or run around the course without their dogs, determining how they can best position themselves and guide their dogs to get the most accurate and rapid path around the numbered obstacles. The handler tends to run a path much different from the dog's path, so the handler can sometimes spend quite a bit of time planning for what is usually a quick run. The walk-through is critical for success because the course's path takes various turns, even U-turns or 270° turns, can cross back on itself, can use the same obstacle more than once, can have two obstacles so close to each other that the dog and handler must be able to clearly discriminate which to take, and can be arranged so that the handler must work with obstacles between himself and the dog, called layering, or at a great distance from the dog. • Agility field right side: The right side of the same agility field showing (clockwise from foreground) the weave poles, the pause table, the A-frame, two winged jumps, the collapsed tunnel (or chute), and a wingless jump. Numbered orange plastic cones next to obstacles indicate the order in which the dog must perform them. 1.4.2 Agility obstacles The regulations of different organizations specify somewhat different rules and dimensions for the construction of obstacles. However, the basic form of most obstacles is the same wherever they are used. Obstacles include the following: Contact obstacles Printed maps of the agility course, called course maps, are often made available to the handlers before they run, to Tunnels help the handlers plan their course strategy. The course map contains icons indicating the position and orienta- Jumps tion of all the obstacles, and numbers indicating the order in which the obstacles are to be taken. Course maps were originally drawn by hand, but nowadays almost all course maps are created using a program called Clean Run Course Designer. Each dog and handler team gets one opportunity together to attempt to complete the course successfully. The dog begins behind a starting line and, when instructed by his handler, proceeds around the course. The handler typically runs near the dog, directing the dog with spoken commands and with body language (the position of arms, shoulders, and feet). Because speed counts as much as accuracy, especially at higher levels of competition, this all takes place at a fullout run on the dog's part and, in places, on the handler's This winged single jump is adjusted in height so that small dogs such as Pembroke Welsh Corgis may compete against similarpart as well. sized dogs. Scoring of runs is based on how many faults are incurred. Penalties can include not only course faults, such as knocking down a bar in a jump, but also time faults, Jump (or hurdle) Two uprights supporting a horizonwhich are the number of seconds over the calculated stantal bar over which the dog jumps. The height is addard course time, which in turn is determined based on justed for dogs of different heights. The uprights the competition level, the complexity of the course, and can be simple stanchions or can have wings of variother factors. ous shapes, sizes, and colors. 16 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES Double and triple jump (or spread jump) Two upfence hurdle, long hurdle, window hurdle, and water rights supporting two or three horizontal bars spread hurdle. forward or back from each other. The double can have parallel or ascending horizontal bars; the triple always has ascending bars. The spread between the Miscellaneous horizontal bars is sometimes adjusted based on the height of the dog. Panel jump Instead of horizontal bars, the jump is a solid panel from the ground up to the jump height, constructed of several short panels that can be removed to adjust the height for different dog heights. A Chinook on a pause table An Australian Shepherd jumping through a tire jump. Broad jump (or long jump) A set of four or five slightly raised platforms that form a broad area over which the dog must jump without setting their feet on any of the platforms. The length of the jump is adjusted for the dog's height. Tire jump A torus shape roughly the size of a tire (18 inches (46 cm) to 24 inches (61 cm) inside diameter), suspended in a frame. The dog must jump through the opening of the“tire"; like other jumps, the height is adjusted for dogs of different sizes. The tire is usually wrapped with tape both for visibility and to cover any openings or uneven places in which the dog could catch. Many organizations now allow A Border Collie demonstrates fast weave poles. or require a so-called displaceable or breakaway tire, where the tire comes apart in some way if the dog hits it hard enough.* [8] Table (or pause table) An elevated square platform about 3-foot-by-3-foot (1-meter-by-1-meter) square Other hurdles UKC agility allows a variety of hurdles onto which the dog must jump and pause, either sitnot found in other agility organizations: bush hurting or in a down position, for a designated period dle, high hurdle, log hurdle, picket fence hurdle, rail of time which is counted out by the judge, usually 1.4. DOG AGILITY 17 about 5 seconds. The height ranges from about 8 1.4.3 Agility scoring and clean runs to 30 inches (20 to 76 cm) depending on the dog's Each organization has its own rules about what constitutes height and sponsoring organization. a fault, and whether one can earn a qualifying score with faulted runs. A completed run that passes the minimum Pause box A variation on the pause table. The pause defined standards for time, faults, points, etc., is referred box is a square marked off on the ground, usually to as a qualifying run and in some cases earns credit towith plastic pipe or construction tape, where the dog wards agility titles. A qualifying run is also referred to as must perform the “pause”behavior (in either a sit a leg. A clean run or clear round is one with no faults. or a down) just as he would on the elevated table. Different organizations place different values on faults, which can include the following: Weave poles Similar to a slalom, this is a series of 5 to 12 upright poles, each about 3 feet (0.91 m) tall and spaced about 24 inches (61 cm) apart (spacing for 1.4.4 Competition classes AKC was 21 inches (53 cm) until it was changed in January 2010. The extra three inches was to re- Given the available set of obstacles and possible faults, lieve stress on the dog's back.), through which the there are many permutations of games, or classes, that one dog weaves. The dog must always enter with the first can play on the agility field. A typical course is laid out pole to his left, and must not skip poles. For many within a 100-by-100-foot (30 by 30 m) area, with roughly dogs, weave poles are one of the most difficult ob- 10 to 20 feet (3.0 to 6.1 m) between obstacles. stacles to master. Judges design their own courses (in NADAC, judges can do so or can select from previously designed courses) usOther obstacles UKC agility allows the following obsta- ing the rules of the sanctioning organization. Each orgacles not found in other agility organizations: swing nization decides which classes are valid for achieving tiplank, sway bridge, and platform jump. NADAC tles and how each must be performed, but there are many also uses a hoop obstacle. A Hoopers course con- similarities. sists entirely of hoops, but hoops may be used in Some of the common types of courses or classes are other courses as well. References for equipment Equipment specifications for various organizations: • AKC (PDF), under “Obstacle Specifications and Performance Requirements”(United States) • ASCA (PDF), in Appendix A “Equipment Specifications”(United States) • CKC, Canadian rules, see Appendix A (Canada) • CPE, follow the “Rules”link (United States) • FCI (PDF), under“Obstacle Specification”(International) • NADAC (North America) • TDAA (North America) • USDAA (North America) • UKC, list of equipment, no specs (North America) • The Kennel Club, partial specs (United Kingdom) • Standard, Regular, or Agility: This is a numbered course consisting of (usually) at least one of each of the three primary contact obstacles (not including the crossover) plus jumps, tunnels, and weave poles of various flavors. A novice course might consist of as few as 15 obstacles; a higher-level course might have 22. The dog must negotiate the obstacles in the correct order within the standard course time (SCT). • Jumpers or Jumping: This numbered course consists primarily of various types of jumps and, depending on the organization, also weave poles and tunnels. The dog must negotiate the obstacles in the correct order within the standard course time (SCT). The dogs achieve their fastest speed on these courses because there are no contact obstacles to slow them down. • Gamblers, FAST, Jackpot, or Joker: An unnumbered course. The game typically consists of two parts, an opening period and the closing period, also known as the gamble, joker, or jackpot. In the opening period, the dog has a certain amount of time in which to do whatever obstacles the handler deems appropriate and accrues points based on the obstacles completed. At the end of the allocated time for the opening period, a horn or whistle blows. At that point, the gamble begins. The dog has a certain small amount of time (about 15 seconds) in which to 18 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES complete a sequence of obstacles designated by the agility course, with the handlers exchanging a baton judge ahead of time. (In FAST, there is no opening between sections. and closing period and this sequence may be taken at any time during the run.) The challenge is that there is a line on the ground past which the handler 1.4.5 Fairness among dogs and handlers must not step, typically paralleling the gamble obstacles and from 5 to 20 feet (1.5 to 6.1 m) away depending on the level of competition. The handler must choose an opening sequence that flows comfortably for the individual dog's skills and experience. The sequence must also be planned so that, when the horn blows, the dog is in a good position to immediately begin the gamble. The greatest challenge is the distance handling – getting the dog to move or even turn away from you. • Snooker: Loosely based on the billiard game of Snooker. The course has at least three red jumps, each numbered 1, and six other obstacles numbered 2 through 7. The dog accumulates points based on the obstacle's number. This also has two parts, an opening sequence and a closing sequence. In the Australian Koolie smooth coat competing in an agility trial. opening sequence, the dog must complete a 1, then any obstacle numbered 2 to 7, a different 1 and any 2 to 7 obstacle (including the one already performed), and yet another different 1 and another 2-7 obstacle. For example, the dog could perform the red on the left for 1 point, the 7-point obstacle, the red in the middle for 1 point and then the 7-pointer again, then the red on the far side of the course and the 7-pointer one more time, for a total of 24 points in the opening. After successfully completing this, the dog must complete the obstacles 2-7, in order, for an additional possible 27 points. Failure to follow these rules exactly (such as knocking a bar or taking 2 reds in a row) results in the dog and handler being A St. Bernard competing in dog agility. whistled off the course. Although each organization has its own rules, all divide • Strategy and entertainment value: The dogs dogs into smaller groups that are close to each other in size might have to negotiate between other obsta- and experience for purposes of calculating winners and cles without taking them or make a difficult qualifying scores: * [9]* [10]* [11] * [12]* [13]* [14]* [15] entry to the obstacle, often combined with a longer distance between the reds and the 7 so • Dogs are measured in height at the peak of their that it consumes more time to do the higherwithers (shoulders). They are then divided into point obstacle in the opening. height groups; for example, dogs measuring between 12 and 16 inches (30 and 41 cm) might compete to• Power & Speed: The course consists of two secgether with the jumps set at a height of 16 inches tions. The first is an untimed “Power”section, (41 cm). This ensures that dogs who might have which features the contact equipment and any of the an advantage on a particular course because of their following at the discretion of the judge setting the size (larger or smaller) keep the advantage to a mincourse: weaves, table, a-frame, spread jump, long imum. jump. If this section is negotiated without accruing any faults, the dog and handler may go on to the • Dogs are further divided into their experience lev“Speed”section, which consists of a timed jumping els. So, for example, there may be competitions for course. 12 inches (30 cm) Novice dogs, 12 inches (30 cm) • Team, Pairs, or Relay: Two or three dog-andhandler teams each execute a portion of a Standard Intermediate dogs, and 12 inches (30 cm) Masters dogs. Dogs typically have to have certain numbers of successes at lower levels before they can move up 1.4. DOG AGILITY 19 to compete with more advanced dogs. Some organizations allow beginner dogs to run on-leash in some situations. • Some organizations divide dogs into additional optional categories because the dogs are older (usually over seven years), need to jump at a lower height than the regular standard, or the like. For example, a veteran's class for older dogs might allow the dogs to jump at a height lower than the standard height and to have more time to complete the course. Otherwise, dogs are not separated by age; they must only be of at least a specified minimum age to compete. • Some organizations divide handlers into additional optional categories, such as junior handlers (usually A mixed-breed dog demonstrates the teeter at an agility class. under 18), handicapped handlers, or senior handlers. Dogs are not separated by breed in agility competitions. Some organizations require that dogs entering its competitions must be purebred, but many organizations allow any sound, able-bodied dog, whether purebred or mixedbreed. Blind dogs and dogs with disabilities judged to make the course run physically dangerous to the dog are generally ineligible for the dog's own safety. Among the major agility associations worldwide, the AKC stands out in its exclusion of deaf dogs from agility competition.* [9] 1.4.6 History Main article: History of dog agility The history of dog agility can be traced to a demonstration at the Crufts Dog Show in the late 1970s in the United Kingdom. Dogs were run around a course designed similar to horse jumping courses during intermission as a way to entertain the audience. It has since spread rapidly around the world, with major competitions held worldwide. 1.4.7 Geography Main article: Geography of dog agility Dog agility is an international dog sport with many different sanctioning organizations and competitions worldwide. 1.4.8 Training Dogs can begin training for agility at any age; however, care is taken when training dogs under a year old so as to not harm their developing joints.* [16] Dogs generally start training on simplified, smaller, or lowered (in height) agility equipment and training aids (such as ladders and wobbling boards to train careful footing);* [17] however, even quickly learning puppies must be finished growing before training on equipment at standard height to prevent injury. Introducing a new dog to the agility obstacles varies in response. Each individual dog learns at his own pace; confident dogs may charge over equipment with little encouragement, while more timid dogs may take weeks to overcome their hesitations with much encouragement. Both scenarios present their own challenges; dogs may be overconfident and sloppy to the point where they have a serious accident, so self-control must be taught.* [16]* [18] Timid dogs need extra support to boost their confidence.* [18] Given the right encouragement, a timid dog can gain confidence through learning the sport itself.* [16]* [18] The size of the dog can also have an effect on training obstacles, particularly with the chute, in which smaller dogs are prone to getting trapped and tangled inside.* [17] Great effort is taken in general to see that the dog is always safe and has a good experience in training for agility so that they do not fear the obstacles, and instead perform them willingly and with enthusiasm.* [17] The teeter-totter (or see-saw) and the weave poles are typically the most challenging obstacles to teach to any dog.* [16] Many dogs are wary of the see-saw's movement, and the weave poles involve a behavior that does not occur naturally to the dog.* [16]* [17] Contact obstacles in general are challenging to train in a manner that ensures that the dog touches the contact zone without sacrificing speed. Whether for competition or recreation, the most important skill for an agility team to learn is how to work together quickly, efficiently, and safely.* [17] Dogs vary greatly in their speed and accuracy of completing a course, as well as in their preferences for obstacles; therefore, the handler must adjust their handling style to suit and support the dog. Training techniques for each piece of equipment varies. For example, the techniques for training the weave poles include using offset poles that gradually move more in line with each other; using poles that tilt outward from 20 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES the base and gradually become upright; using wires or gates around the poles forcing the dog into the desired path; putting a hand in the dog's collar and guiding the dog through while leading with an incentive; teaching the dog to run full speed between two poles and gradually increasing the angle of approach and number of poles; et cetera.* [17] Agility can be trained independently (for instance at home) or with an instructor or club that offers classes. Seasoned handlers and competitors, in particular, may choose to train independently, as structured classes are commonly geared towards novices.* [16] Seasoned handlers often instead look to seminars and workshops that teach advanced handling techniques, and then practice on their own. Common reasons for joining an agility class include: • Access to agility equipment, especially the larger contact obstacles, which can be expensive, difficult English Springer Spaniel to build, and require a lot of space to use.* [18] • Seeking the guidance and expertise of more experi- trial takes place, and another member to be the secretary, enced handlers.* [18] who is responsible for providing competitors with the • Enjoying the social venue that many classes pro- show premium or schedule̶a document that describes the specific competition, summarizes the rules, describes vide.* [18] the trial site, and includes an entry form̶receiving com• Training in a more distracting environment, which pleted entry forms, sending out running orders, producing is helpful in preparation for competition.* [18] running-order lists for the day of competition, and compiling the results from the trial to send to the sanctioning In addition to the technical and educational training, organization. physical training must also be done.* [18] At the very least, the dog must be fit enough to run and jump without The designated chief ring steward or ring manager is recausing stress or injury to its body. The handler can also sponsible for finding and assigning workers, almost albenefit from being physically fit, but with some handling ways volunteers, to perform the myriad tasks involved in styles it is not necessary to keep up with the dog (nor is it putting on a trial. For example, if electronic timing is not possible with very fast dogs).* [18] Being able to handle a being used, each class needs a timer, who ensures that the dog from a distance allows mobility-impaired handlers to dog's running time is recorded, a scribe, who records the participate in the sport en par with mobile handlers. Re- judge's calls as a dog runs the class, and pole setters (or search has also demonstrated health benefits to handlers ring stewards), who ensure that jump bars are reset when they are knocked off and change jump heights for dogs of engaged in dog agility.* [19] different sizes. 1.4.9 Competition process Competition locations Competitions (also called trials or matches or shows) are usually hosted by a specific local club. The club might be devoted solely to dog agility, or it might be primarily a breed club that wants to promote the working abilities of its breed, or it might be a club that hosts many types of dog sports. The club contracts with judges who are licensed by the sanctioning organization and applies to the organization for permission to hold a trial on a specific date or weekend; most trials are two-day weekend events. Agility competitions require considerable space. Each ring is usually at least 100 feet (30 m) on each side, though exact dimensions vary according to the organisations. Competitions can have anything from one up to more than a dozen rings. The ground must be level and, ideally, grassy, although other surfaces are used. In addition, competitors need space to set up quarters for their dogs and gear; when space permits, competitors often bring pop up canopies or screenroom awning tents for shade. Dogs, when not competing, are usually left Key trial jobs to rest in exercise pens, crates, or dog tents familiar and The club designates a member to be the chairperson or enclosed environments in which they can relax and reshow manager, who is responsible for ensuring that the cover between runs. Handlers also bring reflective cloths 1.4. DOG AGILITY to protect their dogs from sun exposure and to calm them down (by covering their crates with the cloths). There also needs to be space for many handlers with dogs on leashes to move freely around the rings without crowding, and space for warming up, exercising, and pottying dogs. Adjacent to the site, parking must be available for all competitors. At weekend or weeklong shows that offer camping, space needs to be provided both for competitors' caravans and tents, and for the small fenced enclosures or gardens that they set up around them. In heavily populated areas, therefore, it is uncommon to find real estate inexpensive enough to devote entirely to agility, so sites are usually rented for the weekend. Even in more rural areas, agility-only sites are uncommon. Popular locations include fairgrounds, large parks, covered horse-riding arenas, and in cold-winter areas, large, empty warehouses in which mats or carpet can be laid. Course design 21 correspond to a grid: for example, if course maps are printed on a grid of 10-foot-by-10-foot squares, the posts that hold the ring ropes marking the course's four sides are often set 10 feet apart. When the course builders finish, the judge walks through the course and double-checks that the obstacles are legal, that they are placed where the judge intended, and that there are no unintended hazards on the course (such as potholes, uneven ground, or mud puddles) around which the course must be adjusted. For many classes, the judge then measures the path through the course to determine the optimal running distance of a typical dog. The judge uses that measurement with a speed requirement determined by the rules to calculate the standard course time, the time under which dogs must complete the course to avoid time faults. For example, if the course is 150 yards (or meters) long, and the rules state that dogs must run the course at a rate of at least 3 yards (or meters) per second, the standard course time would be 50 seconds. Other organizations, though, leave the decision on course time to the judge's discretion Before the trial, each judge designs the courses that he or she will judge at the competition. The sanctioning organization usually reviews and approves the courses to ensure Running a course and determining results that they meet the organization's guidelines. Guidelines include such issues as how far apart obstacles must be, how many turns are allowed (or required) on a course, which obstacles and how many of each must appear on the course, and so on. The rules vary by level of competition and by organization. Building a course and calculating times A Weimaraner jumping an ascending triple-bar spread jump Golden Retriever in an agility competition. Before each class, or the evening before the first class, course builders use course maps provided by the judges to place equipment on the course. The chief course builder is usually an experienced competitor who understands what equipment is legal, how it must be configured, how each must be aligned compared to other obstacles, and can direct several course-building volunteers to efficiently move the equipment into place. To make the job easier, courses are often marked in some way to The judge often holds a briefing for competitors before each class, to review the rules and explain specific requirements for a particular course. For Standard courses for experienced competitors, the judge's briefing is often minimal or dispensed with altogether. For novice handlers in classes with complex rules, the briefings can be much longer. The competitors then walk the course (as described earlier). When the walk-through ends, the gate steward or caller ensures that dogs enter the ring in the running order previously determined by the trial secretary and manages changes to the running order for handlers who might have conflicts with other rings of competition. As each dog and handler team runs the course, the dog is timed either by a person with a stopwatch or with an electronic timer, and the scribe writes the judge's calls and the dog's 22 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES final time on a scribe sheet or ticket, which is then taken 1.4.10 Injuries to the score table for recording. Surveys of handlers indicates that about 1 in 3 dogs incur At the score table, scorekeepers compile the results in a injuries from agility related activities. The most common variety of ways. Some organizations require or encourage types of injuries were (in order) strains, sprains and concomputerized scorekeeping; others require certain types tusions. Locations most commonly injured were shoulof manual score sheets to be filled out. When all the dogs ders, back, phalanges (forelimb/hindlimb) and neck. Inin a given height group, level, and class have run, the score juries were most commonly perceived as being caused table compares run times, faults, and any other requireby interactions with bar jumps (contact), A-frames and ments to determine placements (and, for classes that prodog walk obstacles (contact and/or fall). There were no vide qualifying points towards titles, which dogs earned relationship between the use of warm-up and cool-down qualifying scores). exercises and injuries.* [20]* [21] Each ring might run several classes during a day of competition, requiring multiple course builds, walk throughs, 1.4.11 See also briefings, and so on. • Championship (dog) Awards and Titles • Dog agility worldwide • Dog sports • Dock jumping • Fifteen and Send Time • Rat agility • Show Jumping 1.4.12 References [1] “AAC Rules and Regulations v4.0”. Retrieved April 6, 2011. [2] “AKC Regulations for Agility Trials”. Retrieved December 8, 2009. [3] “NADAC Exhibitor's Handbook”. Retrieved December 8, 2009. [4] “Official UKC Rules and Regulations”. Retrieved April 6, 2011. [5] “USDAA Rules and Regulations”. Retrieved December 8, 2009. A variety of rosette award ribbons from dog agility competitions. Awards are usually given for placements and for qualifying scores. Such awards are often flat ribbons, rosettes, commemorative plaques, trophies, medals, or pins. Some clubs award high-in-trial awards, calculated in various ways, or other special awards for the trial. Dogs who complete their final qualifying scores to become agility champions are often presented with special awards. [6] “CKC Agility Rules and Regulations”. Retrieved May 9, 2014. [7] “2011 Rulebook”. Canine Performance Events, Inc. 2011. Retrieved April 6, 2011. [8] “USDAA news release about tire specifications”. Retrieved December 7, 2011. [9] “AKC Rules (PDF; refer to“Classes, Titles, and Height Divisions”)". Retrieved December 7, 2011. Many Kennel Clubs also award titles to those who man- [10] “ASCA Rules (PDF; refer to“Measuring a Dog's Height” and“ASCA Sanctioned Classes, Divisions & Levels”)". age to qualify enough times in a particular level. Most Retrieved December 7, 2011. clubs require three qualifying scores in any level to get the corresponding title, however, other clubs may require [11] “CPE Rules (PDF; follow Rules and see pages 4, 5, and more or less. 10)". Retrieved December 7, 2011. 1.5. HERDING 23 [12] “FCI Rules (Word; refer to “Tests, Categories, and Classes”)". Retrieved December 7, 2011. [13] “NADAC Rules and FAQs”. Retrieved December 7, 2011. [14] “USDAA Certification Programs”. Retrieved December 7, 2011. [15] “The Kennel Club General Information, Grading Structure, and Measurements”. Retrieved December 7, 2011. [16] Margaret H. Bonham (2000). Introduction to Dog Agility. Barron's Educational Series. ISBN 0-7641-1439-5. [17] Julie Daniels (1991). Enjoying Dog Agility: From Backyard to Competition. Doral Publishing. ISBN 0-94487516-5. A man herding goats in Tunisia [18] Jacqueline O'Neil (1998). All About Agility. Howell Books. ISBN 0-87605-412-2. Some animals instinctively gather together as a herd. A group of animals fleeing a predator will demonstrate herd behavior for protection; while some predators, such as wolves and dogs have instinctive herding abilities derived [20] Cullen, K. L.; Dickey, J. P.; Bent, L. R.; Thomason, J. from primitive hunting instincts.* [1] Instincts in herding J.; Moëns, N. M. M. (2013). “Survey-based analysis of risk factors for injury among dogs participating in agility dogs and trainability can be measured at noncompetitive training and competition events”. Journal of the Ameri- herding tests. Dogs exhibiting basic herding instincts can * can Veterinary Medical Association 243 (7): 1019–1024. be trained to compete in herding and stock dog trials. [1] Sperm whales have also been observed teaming up to herd doi:10.2460/javma.243.7.1019. PMID 24050569. prey in a coordinated feeding behavior.* [2] [19] Dog Agility Exercise Study, University of Massachusetts Dept. of Kinesiology, April 2010. [21] Cullen, K. L.; Dickey, J. P.; Bent, L. R.; Thomason, J. J.; Moëns, N. M. M. (2013). “Internet-based survey of the nature and perceived causes of injury to dogs participating in agility training and competition events”. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 243 (7): 1010– 1018. doi:10.2460/javma.243.7.1010. PMID 24050568. 1.4.13 External links • Agility Association of Canada (AAC) • AKC Rules and Regulations • USDAA Rules and Regulations • NADAC Rules and Regulations Herding is used in agriculture to manage domesticated animals. Herding can be performed by people or trained animals such as herding dogs that control the movement of livestock under the direction of a person.* [3] The people whose occupation it is to herd or control animals often have herd added to the name of the animal they are herding to describe their occupation (shepherd, goatherd, cowherd). These -herds may use herding dogs to assist them and a competitive sport has developed in some countries where the combined skill of man and dog is tested and judged in a Trial such as a Sheepdog trial. Animals such as sheep, camel, yak and goats are mostly reared. They provide milk, meat and other products to the herders and their families. • FCI Agility Regulations • IFCS Agility Regulations • ANKC Agility Trial Rules • The Kennel Club (UK) Agility Regulations 1.5 Herding 1.5.1 Notes [1] Hartnagle-Taylor, Jeanne Joy; Taylor, Ty (2010). Stockdog Savvy. Alpine Publications. ISBN 978-1-57779-1065. [2] http://www.sciencenews.org/view/generic/id/56563/ title/Sperm_whales_may_team_up_to_herd_prey [3] Renna, Christine Hartnagle (2009). Herding Dogs. Ken- nel Club Books. ISBN 978-1-59378-737-0. Herding is the act of bringing individual animals together into a group (herd), maintaining the group and moving the group from place to place̶or any combination of those. While the layperson uses the term “herd- 1.5.2 See also ing”, most individuals involved in the process term it mustering, “working stock”or droving. • Herd (disambiguation) 24 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES • Herder • Herding dogs • Pastoralism 1.6 Hunting dog For the species known as the African hunting dog, Cape hunting dog, or painted hunting dog, see African wild dog. A hunting dog refers to a canine that hunts with or for humans. There are several types of hunting dogs developed for various tasks. The major categories of hunting dogs include hounds, terriers, dachshunds, cur type dogs, and gun dogs. Among these categories further divisions can be made based upon the dogs' skill sets. 1.6.1 Breeds and capabilities used in hunting For a list of breeds of each type, see the detailed articles for each category: Details about the Categories Spaniels definitively fall into two types: ones that seek prey in water and others that seek it on land. Spaniels are the oldest class of gundog in existence, going back at least to the late Renaissance. Flushing spaniels combine hunting, flushing, and retrieving skills. Flushing spaniels that are used in the modern field include the Brittany, the English Springer Spaniel, the slightly smaller Welsh Springer Spaniel, and the field bred American and English Cocker Spaniels. The larger two chiefly are used for retrieveing and flushing game in thick grass or mild underbrush, with the Brittany having working habits closest to later developed pointers. Cocker Spaniels are generally used for thick prickly brush that they can duck, dive and dodge in pursuit of smaller game like rabbits, and Clumbers, Sussex, and Field Spaniels are preferred for their slower, methodical hunting pattern. The American Water Spaniel, Irish Water Spaniel, Kooikerhondje, and the Boykin Spaniel are noted for their water work and do very well in temperate water, with the last being adapted to subtropical swamps. They fall into the water spaniel category. Many of these breeds vary their game according to the desires of the hunter: American Water Spaniels are known to be able to go after animals as big as a large goose in the water or the much smaller prairie chicken out of the water. Boykin Spaniels have a coat more closely adapted to the warmer temperatures of the American South whereas Irish Water Spaniels are adapted for cool, damp conditions, hence the curly coat and whiplike tail of the latter. Like spaniels, hounds generally fall into two types: Sighthounds and scenthounds. The scenthounds are the younger of the two classes. Typical examples of the scenthound family include the Beagle, Bloodhound, members of the Coonhound family, and the Grand Bleu de Gascogne. There is great variety in how this group operates, but the one constant is having some of the strongest noses in dogdom: Bloodhounds have been used for hundreds of years to track both man and beast, sometimes on trails that have been sitting on the ground for days. Coonhounds were originally bred in the American South. They are still used to this day to hunt many different kinds of beasts, ranging in size from the squirrel to the American black bear, so accordingly they are bred for great stamina in multiple terrain, on water and land (all are excellent swimmers,) a loud booming bark that can carry for miles, and a short coat that pairs well with a humid subtropical climate. Beagles have been bred since at least the 16th century as rabbit and fox hunters who will relentlessly pursue the scent of prey even when it goes to ground and were originally intended to work in large packs:they have a gregarious temperament. A Grand Bleu de Gascogne is a very large breed of scenthound that is also quite old: it was a common dog for noblemen to use in their hunting parties and also was a pack hunter; many scenthounds in France were kept by wealthy men to trail quarry on private estates. Sighthounds are different from scenthounds in their methods and adaptations. The long lean head of the sighthound gives it a greater degree of binocular vision, and the body is usually quite slender with an elongated lower spine, giving a double suspension gallop when it runs. In many cases this class is older than the scenthound group: the greyhound, the Scottish Deerhound, and the Saluki have origins going well back into the Middle Ages and earlier. Their speed, agility and visual acuity are particularly adapted for coursing game in open meadows or steppes, and all of them are adapted for running down prey rather than just sniffing for them until they catch up. They are independent in nature, and are worked singly or in a "brace" of two or three dogs. Sighthounds are generally quiet and placid dogs compared to other hunting breeds, but are capable of explosive speed. Rhodesian Ridgebacks are one of the few hound breeds with both capabilities, and though they are not the fastest runners they are notable for having exceptionable endurance. Setters and pointers hunt over long distances to find game birds like members of the pheasant and quail family, using their noses to find the prey and then sneaking up on them in the brush, showing the hunter exactly where the bird is hiding. Most of this family comes from Europe, and would include the Shorthaired, Wirehaired, Shorthaired German Pointers and Weimaraner from Germany, The Viszla from Hungary, Bracco Italiano from Italy, and field bred Irish Setters, Irish Red and White Set- 1.6. HUNTING DOG 25 ters, English Pointers, English Setters, and Gordon Setters from the British Isles. Many in this group share traits with spaniels in terms of the coat they have: it is easier to pick out bits of nettle from a long coat than a short one and the coat itself offers some protection from damp and thorny conditions. tremely popular for killing vermin. Unlike many other hunters, this group did not exclusively work in rural areas: rats were rampant in Victorian era London, Edinburgh, Cardiff, Dublin, Birmingham, Belfast, and Glasgow, and poisons had marginal effects: the rats bred in the dirty conditions of these cities faster than traps could be laid. Water dogs fall into two categories: the retrievers and It became very profitable for working class men to have a multi-purpose. Retrievers are excellent swimmers with profession where they trained small dogs to sniff out and kill as many rats as they could as fast as they could. characteristic webbed feet, and many derive from either Canadian, American, or British stock. Retrievers typi- In fox hunting, they are often paired with hounds should cally have oily coats that help repel icy water, and are prey go to ground, since most breeds of terrier will pull noted for having high intelligence and being very strongly the fox out of its hole and never back down until its master bonded to their masters. The Nova Scotia Duck Tolling calls it off. Members of the bull and terrier subfamily are Retriever is very unusual in the fact that it “tolls"-plays used in the United States and Australia for the hunting of around in the hopes of attracting the attention of water- feral pigs, often paired with scenthounds-their job is to fowl from above and then letting its master shoot the bird, wait until the hounds have found the pig and thereafter whence it retrieves it and goes back in the blind. Golden to charge at it in an explosion of strength and stamina, Retrievers are originally from Scotland: their long, flow- throwing themselves at the pig and keeping it busy until ing double coats make them ideally suited to Scotland's the hunter comes to kill it. They are bred to have great rainy wet climate and their patience on land and in wa- courage and lightning fast reflexes, protecting their master ter is the stuff of legend; they shall wait for a bird for and the other dogs from the sharp tusks of an adult boar hours and will obey their master so long as master re- and the bulldog blood of their ancestors whispers to them wards him with fond affection. Chesapeake Bay Retriev- to bite down and never let go. ers, very popular in the United States, are bred to jump in water after ducks and geese even when there is a coating of ice over the water-they have deep chests meant to act 1.6.2 Gallery as a jackknife that will cut through it when they swim. • Wolf hunt depicted in a 12th-century bestiary Most famous of all is the Labrador, native to an island in Maritime Canada but popular around the world: the field • Medieval women hunting, illustration from a period type Labrador has longer legs and a slimmer frame than manuscript the show type that is better known in Britain, but both show signs of being attracted to water from puppyhood. • Hunting Dog by Li Di, 12th-century Chinese painting Other water dogs are multi purpose. Standard Poodles fall into the water dog category because they originally • Boar hunting, tacuinum sanitatis casanatensis (14th were used by wealthy Germans to hunt ducks; they precentury) date most types of water dogs. Today there are kennels in the United States that have revived the breed for this • Hunting the hart (16th Century) from Turbervile, purpose, with some dogs proving adept hunters at flushcopied from Jaques du Fouilloux. ing bobwhite quail and common pheasant and achieving very high ranks in competitions, sometimes beating the more popular Labrador Retriever. They are highly intel- 1.6.3 References ligent, second only to Border Collies in rank in overall aptitude, and hunters must be very specific in indicating 1.6.4 Further reading what they want when giving commands: they cannot be trained by conventional means and require very concrete • Hunting dogs constellation Canes Venatici signals to indicate what is desired so they may solve the puzzle themselves. They are excellent swimmers whose • Deeley, Martin. “Working Gundogs: An Introcoat requires a simple bath after a swim and a simple duction to Training and Handling. (1990,reprinted cut about an inch off the skin rather than the impracti2002) The Crowood Press. ISBN 1-85223-764-3. cal show clips. Portuguese Water Dogs are medium-sized • Fergus, Charles. Gun Dog Breeds, A Guide to dogs that will retrieve just about anything from the water Spaniels, Retrievers, and Pointing Dogs, The Lyons and have a strong instinct to swim, plus they will guard Press, 2002. ISBN 1-58574-618-5 whatever quarry a hunter keeps: they are one of the only water dogs that were bred to hunt fish. • Roettger, Anthony Z. and Schleider, Benjamin H. Terriers were bred to kill, and are one of the few huntIII. (2004) Urban Gun Dogs: Training flushing dogs ing dogs that have worked in urban environments: many for home and field. The Writer's Collective. ISBN terriers of English, Scottish, and Irish extraction were ex1-59411-050-6 26 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES 1.7 Leash For other uses, see Leash (disambiguation). A leash (also called a lead, lead line or tether) is a rope Tenterfield Terrier on a long leash at a tracking trial • Very short tab leashes; a clip attached to a loop handle or to a short piece of leather with a knot or similar short handle. Allows very close, tight control of a dog in certain competition or training situations. • Short, soft, braided leather leash with a loop handle and a clip to attach to the collar, usually about 4 feet in length, commonly used during obedience training. The softness enables the trainer to fold the leash into a shorter length and the braiding allows a firmer grip. A clip-on leash attached to a dog's collar. or similar material attached to the neck or head of an animal for restraint or control. On the animal, some leashes clip or tie to a collar, harness, or halter, while others go directly around the animal's neck. 1.7.1 Types of leashes • Nylon webbing leash, also known as a tracking/training leash in the UK, usually 4 to 6 feet, with a loop handle and clip, most commonly used for walking dogs casually. • Extended-length webbing leashes, 12 to 30 feet or more, also known as a tracking/training leash in the UK, usually with a loop handle and a clip, primarily for training at a distance or during tracking sessions. • Slip-leash, usually with a loop handle and an adjustable, slipping loop at the other end that goes around the dog's neck. Often used in work or competitions̶such as dog agility̶where the leash must be quickly removed and replaced. • Retractable, a hook on a thin rope that retracts automatically into a large plastic handle, allowing the dog to wander 15 or 25 feet away while keeping the leash taut (in theory preventing it from tangling around obstacles or the dog's legs) but still allowing the handler to reel in the dog for closer control. Nylon webbing leash, a common style For dogs, leashes take many forms; for example: • A simple metal chain. There are also bicycle dog leashes, especially designed for people who enjoy taking their pet in a ride with the bike. The leash is an aluminum tube with a plastic coated cable which runs down through the tube. It extends out of the tube end a couple of feet to allow for ease of movement for the dog. One end connects to the bike and the other to the dog's collar. This keeps them safely away the bike.* [1] Cat leashes and harnesses are also available on the market and are convenient for people who are not comfortable letting their pet free.* [2] 1.7. LEASH 1.7.2 27 Leash laws in the United States of laws and give localities power to make leash law, there are some other states in which leash laws apply statewide. America States that do not have statewide leash laws are Alabama, Alaska, Arizona, Arkansas, California, Colorado, Florida, Georgia, Hawaii, Idaho, Iowa, Kansas, Maryland, Massachusetts, Michigan, Minnesota, Mississippi, Montana, Nevada, New Jersey, New Mexico, North Dakota, Oklahoma, Oregon, Rhode Island, South Carolina, South Dakota, Texas, Utah, Vermont, Virginia, Washington, and Wyoming. In Connecticut, dogs are not permitted to run at large except in the situation of hunting. Still, if the dog has vicious propensities and the owner still allows it to run at large and a person is bitten, the owner can be fined for up to $1,000 and is also liable for 6 months of prison unless the victim has abused the dog and provoked the harmful behavior. Sign near Conneaut Harbor (in Conneaut, Ohio) In Delaware, dogs are not allowed to run at large unless in situations when the owner is present and has control over the pet. An exception is for farm dogs. Also, during the night dogs must be kept in an enclosure from which they cannot escape, firmly secured with a collar or chain or other device, so they cannot stray from the premises,* [3] or are under the reasonable control of the owner or custodian. If an owner does not respect these laws and if the dog bites someone, the owner is subject to civil liability and for fines of up to $1,500. Many cities have passed legislation that requires dogs to be on leash in public areas; in some areas, cats are also required to be restrained (under control) on a leash, in a kennel, or in a cat-proof yard or house. Dogs in the District of Columbia must be kept on a leash as well. They are also not permitted on school grounds when school is in session or on any public recreation area without a leash.* [4] Purposes of a leash include: preventing animals from frightening or biting people or other animals, defecating and urinating in inappropriate places, endangering traffic, digging up lawns, causing other damage, getting lost, and getting away from owners. Leashes also provide a clear method of communication and ensure control during training of dogs. Indiana is one of the states that has a restraint statute, which means that dogs must be restrained at all times. Otherwise, if the dog bites a person when not restrained the owner is subject to civil liability and criminal penalties. (Cite needed; definition of “restraint”needed; discussion of local Indiana ordinances forbidding dog tethering needed.) Dogs are allowed to run at large during the night in Kentucky only if they are accompanied by and under control of their owner. According to the leash laws of Louisiana, dogs are prohibited to run at large at all times of the day. The same law applies in Maine, where the only exception is for hunting dogs. Missouri legislation requires that dogs are kept in leashes that are no longer than 10 feet when they are in state parks or on historic sites.* [5] Also, dogs that have rabies are not permitted to run at large. In Nebraska, dogs may run at large only in counties where the population does not reach 80,000. New Hampshire legislation does not allow dogs to run at large unless they are accompanied by their owner or custodian or when dogs are used for training or are trained In the United States, leash laws are different within each for hunting, herding or exhibitions. state. While some states do not have state-wide leash Cat wearing a harness and leash 28 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES Illinois legislation prohibits owners from walking their collar in order to maintain good control of the dog.* [10] dogs when they are not in a leash. The material of which the leash is made of is not of great Dogs in New York must be restrained or confined at all importance as long as the leash does not show evidence of wear or fraying. Therefore, leashes should be peritimes of the day. odically checked to ensure they are maintained in proper According to the North Carolina law, dogs are allowed to condition. run at large during the night only if they are accompanied An important aspect of dog leashes is their sturdiness. by their owner or a person who has received the owner's Although rope leashes are quite cheap, they are vulnerapermission to do so. ble to chewing and fraying and are not amongst the most Ohio law requires one to “keep the dog under the rea- recommended types of leashes. However, it is considsonable control of some person,”but does not require a ered that a better type of leash is the one made of nylon leash except for “any female dog …at any time the dog because this material provides a bit of elasticity which is is in heat.”There are additional provisions for “danger- meant to result in more comfort for the dog. On the other ous dogs”that have injured a person or killed another hand, nylon leashes can cause chafe or can cut into the dog.* [6] skin of the dog. Pennsylvania legislation states that dogs must be confined Leather leashes are often preferred over the nylon ones or firmly secured or reasonably controlled by a person, because of the resistance of the material and because it within the property of the owner. becomes more flexible with age and it is softer. Leather Tennessee law prohibits dogs to run at large except in is however more prone to be chewed when compared to cases in which dogs are engaged in legal hunting or herd- nylon. ing. The retractable dog leash is one of the most comfortable West Virginia and Wisconsin are states that do not have leashes for the dogs because they allow them to go as far a law that requires dogs to be leashed. Still, they do have as they want as long as the owner does not consider it laws that hold dog owners and keepers liable for all dam- a danger. Retractable leashes are usually made of nylon ages caused by dogs that are permitted to run at large.* [7] and the retractable device is made of plastic or a stronger composite. Although these leashes can be convenient for Different law applies to dangerous dogs and female dogs both the dog and the owner as it allows some control, they as in different states they are prohibited to run at large make it difficult to keep an aggressive dog under control at all times. Also, in states such as Connecticut and which can result in persons or other dogs being attacked. Louisiana, guide dogs must also be leashed.* [8] Aggressive dogs should not be walked with such a leash and also puppies should be kept closely to ensure its protection from various dangers such as cars. 1.7.3 Dog leashes Dog leashes are used to walk the dog in public places. Having the dog wear a leash is a way of protecting the dog and other persons on the street. When walking a dog in crowded places they can get easily confused and lost or they could be involved in car accidents. Also, crowded places might cause them distress which might result in attacking a person. Not to mention that in some areas walking the dog without a leash is against the law. Dog leashes come in variety of designs and colors and they can be made of leather, nylon or other composite materials. Also, the length is one of the important aspects of the leash. The length of the leash must be chosen according to the size of the dog and it is important because it allows a good control. Leashes should not be either too long or too short. Too long leashes do not provide good control of the pet which can result in unpleasant accidents with more aggressive dogs whereas too short leashes are uncomfortable for both the dog and the owner. The perfect leash can restrain the dog but at the same time is not viewed as a punishment for the pet.* [9] Some leashes are made of reflective materials and are suitable for walking the dog at night. They are convenient because they make the dog and the owner much more visible in the traffic, reducing the likelihood of accidents. In India, dog leashes are made from the usual materials, including chain, leather and nylon. Australia is the biggest net importer of Indian made chain leashes, accounting for nearly 22% of the Australian dog leash market. 1.7.4 Cat leashes Cat leashes are used with the purpose of preventing the cat getting lost. Unlike dogs, cats rarely get into fights or attack persons on the street so cat leashes are mainly a safety measure to protect the pet itself. Very often the cat leashes are replaced with harnesses because they avoid the dangers of the leash which include escaping and running away or choking. Cats are more likely to not be willing to be walked in a harness than dogs are. That is why cats are considered to need up to months to be able to adjust High quality dog leashes have a good quality metal clip to wearing a harness. and they can be made of leather, nylon or even chain. Cat leashes come in a variety of colors, designs and modThe metal clip must securely fasten to a metal ring on the els and they are also made of different materials. There 1.8. LIST OF DOG SPORTS 29 are cat leashes made of leather, nylon or rope. Whereas [9] “Dog Collars Leashes”. Retrieved 2010-06-04. the leather leash is one of the best qualities because of the characteristics of the material, it is also one of the most [10] “Dog Leash - How to Select the Best Basic Dog Leash” . Retrieved 2010-06-04. expensive and not very comfortable for the cat at the same time. Nylon cat leashes and harnesses are however more [11] Noah Webster, “Leash”. Dictionary, 1828. elastic and thus more comfortable and also provide more control. 1.8 List of dog sports 1.7.5 Other uses • Among hunters, a collection of three hares “ ( a brace and a half”or tierce) or three creatures of any kind, especially greyhounds, foxes, and deer, is called “a leash”.* [11] Dog sports are activities that involve dogs. • Other types of key leashes include: • Toddler leashes to ensure that young children do not wander far away from their guardians, and are typically attached as a harness to the toddler, while the other end containing the loop is held by the guardian. Toddler leashes are a controversial product and parents have strong opinions about it. • Key leashes and lanyards are used to keep the Training a retrieve in Schutzhund, a dog sport keys attached to handbags. • People enjoying BDSM sometimes uses a leash as a sex toy. 1.7.6 See also • Creance • Fiador (tack) • Child harness • Electronic leash 1.7.7 References [1]“Comparing Bicycle-mounted Leashes”. Retrieved 201005-10. [2] “Cat Leash and Harness Devices”. Retrieved 2010-0510. [3] “Delaware Leash Law”. Retrieved 2010-05-10. [4] “District of Columbia Leash Law”. Retrieved 2010-0510. [5] “Missouri Leash Law”. Retrieved 2010-05-10. There is much discussion about what exactly defines a sport for dogs. Some issues are: • Must a sport be entertaining to watch? Agility, Disc dog, and Dock Jumping are very entertaining to spectators, and often televised. • If a human companion is not actively involved, is it actually a sport? Take greyhound racing, for example, or hunting from a duck blind, from which the dog retrieves the game. • Is 'any' activity a sport if a casual observer does not understand the nature of the competition? For example, in a conformation show, the handler and dog move around a ring for a judge to evaluate the dog's appearance and structure; the skill and knowledge required are not obvious to those uninterested in the sport. This list is intended only to represent anything that anyone is likely to refer to as a dog sport, not to argue its validity as sport. This list is incomplete; you can help by expanding it. [6] “Ohio Leash Law”. Retrieved 2012-04-30. [7] “Wisconsin Leash Law”. Retrieved 2010-05-10. [8] “Overview of State Dog Leash Laws”. Retrieved 201005-10. • Agility • Barn Hunt* [1] 30 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES • Greyhound jockey • Hare coursing • Herding or stock dog • Hunting • Hound trailing • Junior showmanship • Jack Russell hurdle racing An Alaskan Malamute going over an A-frame during a dog agility competition • Lure coursing • Dog mushing • Musical canine freestyle, canine dressage, heelwork to music • Nosework • Obedience training • Protection sports (including Schutzhund, PSA - Protection Sports Association, Service Dogs Of America and French Ring sport) • Rally obedience A Golden Retriever dock jumping into a pool • Retrieving trials • Bikejoring • Schutzhund • Cani cross • Scootering • Caniteering • Sheepdog trials (or herding) • Carting • Sighthound disc sport • Competition obedience • Sighthound racing (including Greyhound and Whippet racing, coursing, and lure coursing) • Conformation showing • Catchball (a variation on flyball) • Degility • Disc dog • Skijoring • Skip dog • Sled dog racing • Dock jumping • Dog surfing • Dog hiking, pack hiking • Terrier racing • Dog scootering • Tracking trials (see also Tracking (dog)) • Earthdog trials • Treibball • Field trials • Water work and water rescue • Flyball • Weight pulling • French Ring sport • Dachshund racing (or Wiener racing) • Greyhound racing • Wheelchair mushing 1.9. OBEDIENCE TRAINING 1.8.1 References [1] Barnhunt.com • Sundance, K. (2010). 101 Ways to Do More With Your Dog: Make Your Dog a Superdog with Sports, Games, Exercises, Tricks, Mental Challenges, Crafts, and Bonding. Beverly, MA: Quarry Books 1.8.2 See also • Racing 31 caring for and living with the dog participates and trains the dog, as they will be the one who will be giving the commands. The relationship and trust between the dog and handler are important for success. Basic or beginner's obedience is typically a short course ranging from six to ten weeks, where it is demonstrated to the handler how to communicate with and train the dog in a few simple commands. With most methods the dog is trained one command at a time. Though there may or may not be a specific word attached to it, walking properly on a leash, or leash control, is often the first training required prior to learning other commands. 1.9 Obedience training Obedience training usually refers to the training of a dog and the term is most commonly used in that context. Obedience training ranges from very basic training, such as teaching the dog to reliably respond to basic commands such as“sit”,“down”,“come”, and“stay”, to high level competition within clubs such as the American Kennel Club, United Kennel Club, and the Canadian Kennel Club, where additional commands, accuracy and performance are scored and judged. Obedience implies compliance with the direction or command given by the handler. For a dog to be considered obedient rather than simply trained in obedience, it must respond reliably each time its handler gives a command. German Shepherd Dog 1.9.1 History Working dogs have always learned to obey commands related to the work that they historically performed, such as when a herding dog moves a flock of animals in response to a shepherd's whistled directions, or a hunting dog searching for (or chasing down) quarry or leaving the treed quarry at the hunter's command. In the twentieth century, formalized dog training originated in military and police applications, and the methods used largely reflected the military approach to training humans. In the middle and late part of the century, however, more research into operant conditioning and positive reinforcement occurred as wild animal shows became more popular. Aquatic mammal trainers used clickers (a small box that makes a loud click when pushed on) to“mark”desired behavior, giving food as a reward. The change in training methods spread gradually into the world of dog training. Today many dog trainers rely heavily on positive reinforcement to teach new behaviors. At a basic level, owners want dogs with which they can pleasantly share a house, a car, or a walk in the park. Some dogs need only a minimum amount of training to learn to eliminate outside (be housebroken), to sit, to lie down, or to come on command (obey a recall). Many other dogs prove more challenging. New dog owners might find training difficult and fail to make progress, because they expect dogs to think and act like humans, and are surprised and baffled when the dogs don't. Dogs that demonstrate the previously mentioned basic skills, as well as walking reasonably well on a leash and a few other minor tasks, can be tested for and earn the American Kennel Club's (AKC) Canine Good Citizen certification. While not a competitive obedience title, a CGC certification demonstrates that the dog is sociable, well behaved, and reliable in public settings.* [1] Some inObedience training is often a prerequisite for or compo- surance companies will waive breed restrictions on dogs nent of other training. with CGCs, and many states have passed resolutions supThe actual training of the dog can be done by anyone, the porting and encouraging CGC certification as a yardstick trainer, owner, or a friend. Typically the individual who is for canine manners and responsible dog ownership. Training a dog in obedience can be an ongoing and lengthy process depending on the dog, the methods used, and the skill and understanding of both the trainer and the handler. The level of obedience the handler wishes to achieve with the dog is also a major factor in the time involved, as is the commitment to training by the handler. 32 1.9.2 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES Dog intelligence and training not trained using harsh corrective methods, as this training can be psychologically harmful to the dog and result in further behavioral issues.* [2]* [3] 1.9.3 Commands The specific command word is not important, but consistency in usage is. There are certain commands that are accepted as standard and commonly used. Basic commands • Sit: The dog is in a sitting position. • Down: A dog is typically down when its elbows (front feet) and hocks (rear legs) are touching the ground or floor. • Heel: The dog's head or shoulder is parallel to the handler's leg on the left side of the handler. • Come or Here: (referred to as the recall) “Call your dog”equals “come”or “here”. • Stay: The dog must remain in the position (sit, down, stand) and location under which the command was given until it is released by the handler. A White German Shepherd ready to obey. Certain breeds, such as Doberman Pinschers, German Shepherds, Border Collies, Labrador Retrievers and Golden Retrievers, have reputations as being easier to train than others, such as some hounds and sled dogs. Dogs that have been bred to perform one task to the exclusion of all others (such as the Bloodhound or Husky), or that have been bred to work independently from their handler (such as terriers), may be particularly challenging with obedience training. Dog intelligence is exhibited in many different ways, and a dog that might not be easy to train might nonetheless be quite adept at figuring out how to open kitchen cabinets or to escape from the yard. Novice dog owners need to consider a dog's trainability as well as its energy level, exercise requirements, and other factors before choosing a new pet. Very high intelligence is not necessarily a good thing in a companion dog, as smart dogs can require extensive daily mental stimulation if they are not to become bored and destructive. No breed is impossible to obedience train, but novice owners might find training some breeds quite difficult. The capacity to learn basic obedience̶and even complicated behavior̶is inherent in all dogs. Some breeds may require more patience or creativity in training than others. Individual dogs that exhibit fearful or anxious behaviors should also be handled with greater care, and especially Advanced commands • Stop – a dog that will simply stop whatever it is doing and lie down on command no matter how far it is from its keeper is a dog that can be taken anywhere. • Back up – keepers of large dogs or dogs with a reputation for aggressiveness can make strangers more comfortable by teaching the dog to back up on command. • Growl – the inverse of backing up. Some owners teach non-aggressive dogs to growl on a subtle command – not the word growl, usually a small hand gesture – as a way of letting strangers know that you and your dog value being left alone. • Shake - Directs the dog to shake whole body. Generally used after bathing or swimming to prevent dog from soaking owner. • Shake Hands or Shake - Directs dog to lift paw and place it in the hand of the owner as if shaking hands. • Steady – keep near by. The dog can walk free, but not dash off. • Stand – dog stands still. Useful for grooming. Many dogs are groomed frequently and need to stand quietly during the process. 1.9. OBEDIENCE TRAINING • Go to bed, kennel, or get in: Directs the dog to go to its bed or its crate and to remain there until released. The dog has freedom of movement in that location to stand up, turn around, or lie down, unlike when placed in a Stay. Useful to keep a dog out from underfoot and safe in a busy or complicated situation. • Drop or drop it: Dogs pick up all sorts of things, some of which they shouldn't have. A dog that drops anything on command, no matter how attractive (and “attractive”to a dog can be “rotten and smelly”to a human), is a dog under control that the owner can prevent from eating dangerous items or from destroying valued personal property. 33 Flat collar Flat collars are commonly used in clicker training and other non-correction-based training, such as puppy kindergarten. They are also effective in training small dogs, however they tend to lift the dog off the ground when giving corrections while the dog is distracted or in high adrenal mode. They are typically made of nylon or leather, and fasten with a buckle or quickrelease connection. Slip collar Slip collars (commonly called choke chain or check chains) are made of metal links or rolled material such as nylon or leather. A metal ring is at each end. Historically, slip collars have been used as a matter of course, mostly in North America and the UK. In the last few decades use of these collars has declined. Correctly • Leave it: An adjunct to Drop, directing the dog to used, the collar should make a quick clicking not zipping not touch an item. Also useful before the dog has sound when quickly snapped and released to startle or get picked anything up. Leave it is also used in con- the attention of the dog and indicate to the handler that junction with Take it. the technique was a swift jerk not a choke. The idea is not to strangle the dog, though this can happen if the collar is • Take it: The dog leaves a desired object, such improperly used. as a toy or treat, untouched until given this command. Alternatively, the dog takes and holds an object which it has no interest in. This can protect an Martingale collar Martingale collars (also called owner's, visitor's, or child's fingers. limited-slip collars) are usually made of flat nylon with a smaller fixed-length section (made of either nylon or a • Give: The dog has an object in its mouth and“gives” short length of chain) that, when pulled on by the leash, it to its owner by releasing the object into the owner's shortens up tightening the collar around the dog's neck, hand. Object of choice in training is usually a lightto a limited extent. When properly fitted, martingales weight dumbbell or a glove. This is useful for when are looser than flat-buckle collars when not tightened, and your dog has one of your belongings and you want it less severely corrective than slip collars when tightened. back before the dog hides it or chews it up. • Speak: A dog, when taught this command, will bark Prong collars Prong collars (also called 'pinch colonce (or more) when told to do so. lars') are a series of chain links with blunted open ends • Roll Over: When taught this command a dog will turned towards the dog's neck. The design of the prong collar is such that it has a limited circumference unlike lie down, roll over, and stand back up. slip collars which do not have a limit on how far they • Attack: A dog will attack something (or someone) can constrict on a dog's neck. The limited traction of the when told to do so. Common commands are either martingale chain combined with the angle of the prongs prevents the prongs moving close enough to pinch. The “Attack”or “Sic'em”. collar is designed to prevent the dog from pulling by ap• Fetch: A dog will retrieve a thrown object (usually plying pressure at each point against the dog's neck. a ball or a stick) and bring it back to the one who Prong collars must never be turned inside out (with the threw it. prongs facing away from the dog's skin), as this may cause • Place : The dog is trained to go to a certain place injury against the body and head. Plastic tips are occaand stay there until released, usually a place in the sionally placed on the ends of the prongs to protect against house selected by owner. tufts forming in the fur or, in the case of low quality manufactured collars with rough chisel cut ends, puncturing • With me: used when walking your dog to keep them the skin. Like the slip collar, the prong collar is placed at your side and with your pace. high on the dog's neck, just behind the ears, at the most sensitive point. 1.9.4 Training devices Collars Main article: Dog collar Some dogs can free themselves from prong collars with large wire looped sides by shaking their head so that the links pop out, so some trainers have come to use a second collar (usually an oversize slip collar) in addition to the prong collar so when this happens the dog does not run loose. 34 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES Shock collars Shock collars (also known as E-collars) Schutzhund dog training. Bite tugs are perfect for puptransmit a remote signal from a control device the handler pies but can be used for training adult dogs as well. operates to the collar. An electrical shock is transmitted by the handler remotely, at varying degrees of intensity, from varying distances depending on range frequency. It 1.9.5 Competitive obedience is also done automatically in the bark electronic collar to stop excessive barking, and invisible fence collar when the dog strays outside its boundary. Shock collars are widely used in some areas of the world and by some dog obedience professionals.,.* [4] Some dog training associations, veterinary associations and kennel clubs condemn their use.* [5] Other devices Leash Main article: Leash The leash or lead is used to connect the dog to the handler, lead the dog, as well as to control the dog in urban areas. Most communities have laws which prohibit dogs from running at large. They may be made of any material such as nylon, metal or leather. A six foot length is commonly used for walking and in training classes, though leashes come in lengths both shorter and longer. A long line (also called a lunge line) can be 3 metres (ten feet) or more in length, and are often used to train the dog to come when called from a distance. Clicker Main article: Clicker The clicker is a small hand-held device that makes a distinct, short sound to mark a desired behavior. (See clicker training for a more detailed discussion of this methodology.) It has gained popularity in recent years as being a means of training that does not involve physically correcting the dog, though it may be used in conjunction with these methods. This Smooth Collie retrieves an obedience dumbbell made of wood; others are made of metal. Main article: obedience trial For dog owners who enjoy competition and relish the opportunity to work as a highly tuned team with their dogs, competitive obedience trials are available. Dogs can earn obedience titles, including an obedience championship. In competition, merely sitting, lying down, or walking on a leash are insufficient. The dog and handler must perform the activities off leash and in a highly stylized and carefully defined manner. For example, on a recall, the dog must come directly to the handler, without sniffing or veering to one side, and must sit straight in front of the handler, not at an angle or off to one side or the other. Training for obedience competitions builds on basic obedience training. The United Kennel Club (UKC), the Australian National Kennel Council (ANKC), the American Kennel Club (AKC) and the Australian Shepherd Dog Club of America (ASCA) are some of the organizations which offer Head halters are an alternative to collars that works simititles in Competition Obedience. larly to a horse halter. The halter fits over the dog's snout and behind its head (leading it to sometimes be mistaken AKC obedience titles include: Companion Dog (CD), for a muzzle). Halters reduce the dog's ability to success- Companion Dog Excellent (CDX), Utility Dog (UD), fully pull on the leash, but do not eliminate it. If the halter Utility Dog Excellent (UDX), and Obedience Trial is used with a sharp jerk on the leash, neck injury to the Champion (OTCH). dog may result, but used correctly head halters have not In recent years, a new form of Obedience competition, been shown to cause harm. known as Rally Obedience, has become very popular. It Head halter Main article: Dog collar was originally devised by Charles L.“Bud”Kramer from the obedience practice of“doodling”- doing a variety of Dog bite tug Main article: Dog bite tug interesting warmup and freestyle exercises. Rally Obedience is designed to be a “bridge”, or intermediate step, Dog training bite tug is a tool usually used as retrieve between the CGC certification and traditional Obedience developing skills. It is used for police, military and competition. 1.10. OPERANT CONDITIONING 35 Unlike regular obedience, instead of waiting for the judge's orders, the competitors proceed around a course of designated stations with the dog in heel position. The course consists of 10 to 20 signs that instruct the team what to do. Unlike traditional obedience, handlers are allowed to encourage their dogs during the course. Obedience for other purposes There are many reasons for training dogs beyond the level required for basic companionship. For example, assistance dogs must obey their“sit”and“down”commands perfectly at all times, but they do not have to conform to the rigid rules of competitive obedience. Dogs competing in dog sports, such as flyball, agility or Schutzhund, must be trusted in an open field, off leash and surrounded by other people, dogs, hot dogs, and flying Diagram of operant conditioning discs. This requires more focused attention on the owner and a better recall than that found in most household companion dogs, and more advanced training than that re- 1.10 Operant conditioning quired for formal obedience. 1.9.6 See also • Clicker training • Dog sports • Dog training • The Intelligence of Dogs • Musical canine freestyle • Rally obedience (Rally-O) Operant conditioning, sometimes referred to as instrumental learning, is a method of learning that occurs through reinforcements and punishments for behavior. It encourages the subject to associate desirable or undesirable outcomes with certain behaviors. Instrumental conditioning was first discovered and published by Jerzy Konorski and was also referred to as Type II reflexes. Mechanisms of instrumental conditioning suggest that the behavior may change in form, frequency, or strength. The expressions“operant behavior”and“respondent behavior”were popularized by B.F. Skinner who worked on reproduction of Konorskiʼs experiments. Operant behavior means that“a response is followed by a reinforcing stimulus”. Operant conditioning is distinguished from classical conditioning (or respondent conditioning) in that operant con1.9.7 References ditioning deals with the reinforcement and punishment [1] “About Canine Good Citizen®". American Kennel Club. to change behavior. Operant behavior operates on the environment and is maintained by its antecedents and Retrieved 31 March 2015. consequences, while classical conditioning is maintained [2] Laviolette, Niki (29 March 2015). “Key points to re- by conditioning of reflexive (reflex) behaviors, which member when training a dog”. Tribune Star. Retrieved are elicited by antecedent conditions. Behaviors condi31 March 2015. tioned through a classical conditioning procedure are not maintained by consequences.* [1] They both, however, [3] “Natural Dog Obedience Training”. K9 Basics. 13 Ocform the core of behavior analysis and have grown into tober 2014. Retrieved 31 March 2015. professional practices. Operant conditions are simple to understand, after trial and error Learning is achieved. A [4] The Facts About Modern Electronic Training Devices reward for overcoming an obstacle can give the inner mo[5] Shock Collars - The Shocking Truth, The Association of tivation needed to continue with success. Pet Dog Trainers 1.9.8 External links 1.10.1 Historical notes • AKC Obedience Regulations 36 Thorndike's law of effect Main article: Law of effect Operant conditioning, sometimes called instrumental learning, was first extensively studied by Edward L. Thorndike (1874–1949), who observed the behavior of cats trying to escape from home-made puzzle boxes.* [2] When first constrained in the boxes, the cats took a long time to escape. With experience, ineffective responses occurred less frequently and successful responses occurred more frequently, enabling the cats to escape in less time over successive trials. In his law of effect, Thorndike theorized that behaviors followed by satisfying consequences tend to be repeated and those that produce unpleasant consequences are less likely to be repeated. In short, some consequences strengthened behavior and some consequences weakened behavior. Thorndike produced the first known animal learning curves through this procedure.* [3] CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES Many of Skinner's writings are devoted to the application of operant conditioning to human behavior.* [10] In 1957, Skinner published Verbal Behavior,* [11] which extended the principles of operant conditioning to language, a form of human behavior that had previously been analyzed quite differently by linguists and others. Skinner defined new functional relationships such as“mands”and “tacts”to capture the essentials of language, but he introduced no new principles, treating verbal behavior like any other behavior controlled by its consequences, which included the reactions of the speaker's audience. 1.10.2 Tools and procedures To shape behavior: antecedents and consequences Antecedents as well as the following consequences: reinforcement and punishment are the core tools of operant conditioning. It is important to realize that some terminology in operant conditioning is used in a way that is different from everyday use. Skinner “Antecedent stimuli”occurs before a behavior happens. Main article: B. F. Skinner “Reinforcement”and“punishment”refer to their effect on the desired behavior. 1. Reinforcement increases the probability of a behavB.F. Skinner (1904–1990) often referred to as the father ior being expressed. of operant conditioning. His work is most often cited in connection with this topic. His book “The Behavior of 2. Punishment reduces the probability of a behavior Organisms”,* [4] published in 1938, initiated his lifelong being expressed study of operant conditioning and its application to human and animal behavior. Following the ideas of Ernst Mach, Skinner rejected Thorndike's reference to unob- “Positive”and“negative”refer to the presence or absence servable mental states such as satisfaction, building his of the stimulus. analysis on observable behavior and its equally observable consequences.* [5] 1. Positive is the addition of a stimulus To implement his empirical approach, Skinner invented 2. Negative is the removal or absence of a stimulus (ofthe operant conditioning chamber in which subjects such ten adverse) as pigeons and rats were isolated from extraneous stimuli and free to make one or two simple, repeatable responses.* [6] This was similar to Thorndikeʼs puzzle box There is an additional procedure and became known as the Skinner box. Another invention, the cumulative recorder, produced a graphical 1. Extinction is caused by the lack of any consequence record of these responses from which response rates could following a behavior. When a behavior is inconsebe estimated. These records were the primary data that quential (i.e., producing neither favorable nor unfaSkinner and his colleagues used to explore the effects on vorable consequences) it will occur less frequently. response rate of various reinforcement schedules.* [7] A When a previously reinforced behavior is no longer reinforcement schedule may be defined as “any procereinforced with either positive or negative reinforcedure that delivers reinforcement to an organism according ment, it leads to a decline (extinction) in that behavto some well-defined rule”.* [8] Reinforcement is known ior. as“behavior which is reinforced tends to be repeated (i.e. strengthened); behavior which is not reinforced tends to die out-or be extinguished (i.e. weakened).”The effects This creates a total of five basic consequences of schedules became, in turn, the basic experimental data from which Skinner developed his account of operant 1. Positive reinforcement (reinforcement): Occurs conditioning. He also drew on many less formal obserwhen a behavior (response) is followed by a stimvations of human and animal behavior.* [9] ulus that is appetitive or rewarding, increasing the 1.10. OPERANT CONDITIONING frequency of that behavior. In the Skinner box experiment, a stimulus such as food or a sugar solution can be delivered when the rat engages in a target behavior, such as pressing a lever. This procedure is usually called simply reinforcement. 2. Negative reinforcement (escape): Occurs when a behavior (response) is followed by the removal of an aversive stimulus, thereby increasing that behavior's frequency. In the Skinner box experiment, negative reinforcement can be a loud noise continuously sounding inside the rat's cage until it engages in the target behavior, such as pressing a lever, upon which the loud noise is removed. 3. Positive punishment (punishment) (also called “Punishment by contingent stimulation”): Occurs when a behavior (response) is followed by a stimulus, such as introducing a shock or loud noise, resulting in a decrease in that behavior. Positive punishment is sometimes a confusing term, as it denotes the“addition”of a stimulus or increase in the intensity of a stimulus that is aversive (such as spanking or an electric shock). This procedure is usually called simply punishment. 4. Negative punishment (penalty) (also called“Punishment by contingent withdrawal”): Occurs when a behavior (response) is followed by the removal of a stimulus, such as taking away a child's toy following an undesired behavior, resulting in a decrease in that behavior. 5. Extinction: Occurs when a behavior (response) that had previously been reinforced is no longer effective. For example, a rat is first given food many times for lever presses. Then, in “extinction”, no food is given. Typically the rat continues to press more and more slowly and eventually stops, at which time lever pressing is said to be “extinguished.” It is important to note that actors are not spoken of as being reinforced, punished, or extinguished; it is the actions that are reinforced, punished, or extinguished. Additionally, reinforcement, punishment, and extinction are not terms whose use is restricted to the laboratory. Naturally occurring consequences can also be said to reinforce, punish, or extinguish behavior and are not always delivered by people. Some other common terms and procedures • Escape and avoidance In escape learning, a behavior terminates an (aversive) stimulus. For example, shielding one's eyes from sunlight terminates the (aversive) stimulation of bright light in one's eyes. In avoidance learning, the behavior precedes and prevents an (aversive) stimulus, for example putting on sun glasses before going outdoors. Because, in 37 avoidance, the stimulation does not occur, avoidance behavior seems to have no means of reinforcement. Indeed this non-occurrence of the stimulus has been a problem for reinforcement theory, which has been dealt with in various ways. See section on avoidance learning below. • Noncontingent reinforcement refers to delivery of reinforcing stimuli regardless of the organism's behavior. Noncontingent reinforcement may be used in an attempt to reduce an undesired target behavior by reinforcing multiple alternative responses while extinguishing the target response.* [12] As no measured behavior is identified as being strengthened, there is controversy surrounding the use of the term noncontingent “reinforcement”.* [13] • Schedules of reinforcement Schedules of reinforcement are rules that control the delivery of reinforcement. The rules specify either the time that reinforcement is to be made available, or the number of responses to be made, or both. • Fixed interval schedule: Reinforcement occurs following the first response after a fixed time has elapsed after the previous reinforcement. • Variable interval schedule: Reinforcement occurs following the first response after a variable time has elapsed from the previous reinforcement. • Fixed ratio schedule: Reinforcement occurs after a fixed number of responses have been emitted since the previous reinforcement. • Variable ratio schedule: Reinforcement occurs after a variable number of responses have been emitted since the previous reinforcement. • Continuous reinforcement: Reinforcement occurs after each response.* [14] • Discrimination, generalization & context. Most behavior is under stimulus control. Several aspects of this may be distinguished: •“Discrimination”typically occurs when a response is reinforced only in the presence of a specific stimulus. For example, a pigeon might be fed for pecking at a red light and not at a green light; in consequence, it pecks at red and stops pecking at green. Many complex combinations of stimuli and other conditions have been studied; for example an organism might be reinforced on an interval schedule in the presence of one stimulus and on a ratio schedule in the presence of another. •“Generalization”is the tendency to respond to stimuli that are similar to a previously trained 38 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES discriminative stimulus. For example, having been trained to peck at “red”a pigeon might also peck at “pink”, though usually less strongly. •“Context”refers to stimuli that are continuously present in a situation, like the walls, tables, chairs, etc. in a room, or the interior of an operant conditioning chamber. Context stimuli may come to control behavior as do discriminative stimuli, though usually more weakly. Behaviors learned in one context may be absent, or altered, in another. This may cause difficulties for behavioral therapy, because behaviors learned in the therapeutic setting may fail to occur elsewhere. Operant conditioning to change human behavior Researchers have found the following protocol to be effective when they use the tools of operant conditioning to modify human behavior: 1. State goal (aims for the study) That is, clarify exactly what changes are to be brought about. For example, “reduce weight by 30 pounds.” 2. Monitor behavior (log conditions) Keep track of behavior so that one can see whether the desired effects are occurring. For example, keep a chart of daily weights. 3. Reinforce desired behavior (give reward for proper behavior) For example, congratulate the individual on weight losses. With humans, a record of behavior may serve as a reinforcement. For example, when a participant sees a pattern of weight loss, this may reinforce continuance in a behavioral weight-loss program. A more general plan is the token economy, an exchange system in which tokens are given as rewards for desired behaviors. Tokens may later be exchanged for a desired prize or rewards such as power, prestige, goods or services. 4. Reduce incentives to perform undesirable behavior For example, remove candy and fatty snacks from kitchen shelves. 1.10.3 petite”for that source of stimulation has been satisfied. The opposite effect will occur if the individual becomes deprived of that stimulus: the effectiveness of a consequence will then increase. If someone is not hungry, food will not be an effective reinforcer for behavior. Satiation is generally only a potential problem with primary reinforcers, those that do not need to be learned such as food and water.* [15] 2. Immediacy: After a response, how immediately a consequence is then felt determines the effectiveness of the consequence. More immediate feedback will be more effective than less immediate feedback. If someone's license plate is caught by a traffic camera for speeding and they receive a speeding ticket in the mail a week later, this consequence will not be very effective against speeding. But if someone is speeding and is caught in the act by an officer who pulls them over, then their speeding behavior is more likely to be affected.* [16] 3. Contingency: If a consequence does not contingently (reliably, or consistently) follow the target response, its effectiveness upon the response is reduced. But if a consequence follows the response consistently after successive instances, its ability to modify the response is increased. The schedule of reinforcement, when consistent, leads to faster learning. When the schedule is variable the learning is slower. Extinction is more difficult when learning occurs during intermittent reinforcement and more easily extinguished when learning occurs during a highly consistent schedule.* [15] 4. Size: This is a “cost-benefit”determinant of whether a consequence will be effective. If the size, or amount, of the consequence is large enough to be worth the effort, the consequence will be more effective upon the behavior. An unusually large lottery jackpot, for example, might be enough to get someone to buy a one-dollar lottery ticket (or even buying multiple tickets). But if a lottery jackpot is small, the same person might not feel it to be worth the effort of driving out and finding a place to buy a ticket. In this example, it's also useful to note that “effort”is a punishing consequence. How these opposing expected consequences (reinforcing and punishing) balance out will determine whether the behavior is performed or not. Factors that alter the effectiveness of consequences The majority of these factors exist because of various bi- ological reasons. The biological purpose of the Principle of Satiation is to maintain the organism's homeostasis (an organismʼs ability to maintain a stable internal environment). When an organism has been deprived of sugar, for example, the effectiveness of the taste of sugar as a reinforcer is high. However, as the organism reaches or ex1. Satiation/Deprivation: The effectiveness of a con- ceeds their optimum blood-sugar levels, the taste of sugar sequence will be reduced if the individual's “ap- becomes less effective, perhaps even aversive. When using consequences to modify a response, the effectiveness of a consequence can be increased or decreased by various factors. These factors can apply to either reinforcing or punishing consequences. 1.10. OPERANT CONDITIONING The Principles of Immediacy and Contingency exist for neurochemical reasons. When an organism experiences a reinforcing stimulus, dopamine pathways in the brain are activated. This network of pathways “releases a short pulse of dopamine onto many dendrites, thus broadcasting a rather global reinforcement signal to postsynaptic neurons.”* [17] This allows recently activated synapses to increase their sensitivity to efferent (conducted or conducting outward or away from something) signals, thus increasing the probability of occurrence for the recent responses that preceded the reinforcement. These responses are, statistically, the most likely to have been the behavior responsible for successfully achieving reinforcement. But when the application of reinforcement is either less immediate or less contingent (less consistent), the ability of dopamine to act upon the appropriate synapses is reduced. 1.10.4 Operant variability Operant variability is what allows a response to adapt to new situations. Operant behavior is distinguished from reflexes in that its response topography (the form of the response) is subject to slight variations from one performance to another. These slight variations can include small differences in the specific motions involved, differences in the amount of force applied, and small changes in the timing of the response. If a subject's history of reinforcement is consistent, such variations will remain stable because the same successful variations are more likely to be reinforced than less successful variations. However, behavioral variability can also be altered when subjected to certain controlling variables.* [18] 1.10.5 Avoidance learning In avoidance learning an organism's behavior is reinforced by the termination or prevention of an (assumed aversive) stimulus. There are two kinds of commonly used experimental settings: discriminated and free-operant avoidance learning. Discriminated avoidance learning In discriminated avoidance learning, a novel stimulus such as a light or a tone is followed by an aversive stimulus such as a shock (CS-US, similar to classical conditioning). During the first trials (called escape-trials) the animal usually experiences both the CS (Conditioned Stimulus) and the US (Unconditioned Stimulus), showing the operant response to terminate the aversive US. During later trials, the animal will learn to perform the response during the presentation of the CS thus preventing the aversive US from occurring. Such trials are called “avoidance trials.” 39 Free-operant avoidance learning In this experimental session, no discrete stimulus is used to signal the occurrence of the aversive stimulus. Rather, the aversive stimulus (mostly shocks) are presented without explicit warning stimuli. There are two crucial time intervals determining the rate of avoidance learning. This first one is called the S-S-interval (shock-shock-interval). This is the amount of time which passes during successive presentations of the shock (unless the operant response is performed). The other one is called the RS-interval (response-shock-interval) which specifies the length of the time interval following an operant response during which no shocks will be delivered. Note that each time the organism performs the operant response, the RS-interval without shocks begins anew. Two-process theory of avoidance This theory was originally proposed in order to explain discriminated avoidance learning, in which an organism learns to avoid an aversive stimulus by escaping from a signal for that stimulus. The theory assumes that two processes take place: a) Classical conditioning of fear. During the first trials of the training, the organism experiences the pairing of a CS with an aversive US. The theory assumes that during these trials an association develops between the CS and the US through classical conditioning and, because of the aversive nature of the US, the CS comes to elicit a conditioned emotional reaction (CER) – “fear.” b) Reinforcement of the operant response by fear-reduction. As a result of the first process, the CS now signals fear; this unpleasant emotional reaction serves to motivate operant responses, and those responses that terminate the CS are reinforced by fear termination. Although, after this training, the organism no longer experiences the aversive US, the term “avoidance”may be something of a misnomer, because the theory does not say that the organism “avoids”the US in the sense of anticipating it, but rather that the organism “escapes”an aversive internal state that is caused by the CS. 1.10.6 Four term contingency Applied behavior analysis, which is the name of the discipline directly descended from Skinner's work, holds that behavior is explained in four terms: conditioned stimulus (S* C), a discriminative stimulus (S* d), a response (R), and a reinforcing stimulus (S* rein or S* r for reinforcers, sometimes S* ave for aversive stimuli).* [19] 40 1.10.7 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES Operant hoarding Operant hoarding is a referring to the choice made by a rat, on a compound schedule called a multiple schedule, that maximizes its rate of reinforcement in an operant conditioning context. More specifically, rats were shown to have allowed food pellets to accumulate in a food tray by continuing to press a lever on a continuous reinforcement schedule instead of retrieving those pellets. Retrieval of the pellets always instituted a one-minute period of extinction during which no additional food pellets were available but those that had been accumulated earlier could be consumed. This finding appears to contradict the usual finding that rats behave impulsively in situations in which there is a choice between a smaller food object right away and a larger food object after some delay. See schedules of reinforcement.* [20] 1.10.8 1.10.10 Questions about the law of effect Biological correlates of operant con- A number of observations seem to show that operant behavior can be established without reinforcement in the ditioning The first scientific studies identifying neurons that responded in ways that suggested they encode for conditioned stimuli came from work by Mahlon deLong* [21]* [22] and by R.T. Richardson.* [22] They showed that nucleus basalis neurons, which release acetylcholine broadly throughout the cerebral cortex, are activated shortly after a conditioned stimulus, or after a primary reward if no conditioned stimulus exists. These neurons are equally active for positive and negative reinforcers, and have been demonstrated to cause plasticity in many cortical regions.* [23] Evidence also exists that dopamine is activated at similar times. There is considerable evidence that dopamine participates in both reinforcement and aversive learning.* [24] Dopamine pathways project much more densely onto frontal cortex regions. Cholinergic projections, in contrast, are dense even in the posterior cortical regions like the primary visual cortex. A study of patients with Parkinson's disease, a condition attributed to the insufficient action of dopamine, further illustrates the role of dopamine in positive reinforcement.* [25] It showed that while off their medication, patients learned more readily with aversive consequences than with positive reinforcement. Patients who were on their medication showed the opposite to be the case, positive reinforcement proving to be the more effective form of learning when the action of dopamine is high. 1.10.9 and findings to the behavior of humans in the marketplace. One concept that encompasses both of economics and instrumental conditioning is consumer demand. With consumer demand, the focus is on the price of the commodity and the amount purchased. The degree to which price influences consumption is defined as being the elasticity of demand. Certain commodities are more elastic than others. Price change in certain foods can affect the amount bought, while gasoline and essentials seem to be less effected by price changes. For these examples, gasoline and essentials would be less elastic than certain foods like cake and candy. On a graph model representation, something less elastic would not be stretched out as far as a commodity that's consumption fluctuates greatly due to the price.* [26] Operant conditioning in economics Main article: Consumer demand tests (animals) Further information: Behavioral economics Both psychologists and economists have become interested in applications of operant conditioning concepts sense defined above. Most cited is the phenomenon of autoshaping (sometimes called“sign tracking”), in which a stimulus is repeatedly followed by reinforcement, and in consequence the animal begins to respond to the stimulus. For example, a response key is lighted and then food is presented. When this is repeated a few times a pigeon subject begins to peck the key even though food comes whether the bird pecks or not. Similarly, rats begin to handle small objects, such as a lever, when food is presented nearby.* [27]* [28] Strikingly, pigeons and rats persist in this behavior even when pecking the key or pressing the lever leads to less food (omission training).* [29]* [30] These observations and others appear to contradict the law of effect, and they have prompted some researchers to propose new conceptualizations of operant reinforcement (e.g.* [31]* [32]* [33] A more general view is that autoshaping is an instance of classical conditioning; the autoshaping procedure has, in fact, become one of the most common ways to measure classical conditioning. In this view, many behaviors can be influenced by both classical contingencies (stimulus-reinforcement) and operant contingencies (response-reinforcement), and the experimenterʼs task is to work out how these interact.* [34] 1.10.11 See also • Animal testing • Applied behavior analysis (ABA; application of operant and classical conditioning) • Behavioral contrast • Behaviorism (philosophy behind behavior analysis) • Behavior modification (old expression for ABA; modifies behavior with consequences not antecedents) 1.10. OPERANT CONDITIONING • Biofeedback • Child grooming • Cognitivism (psychology) (theory of internal mechanisms without reference to behavior) • Consumer demand tests (animals) • Educational psychology • Educational technology • Experimental analysis of behavior (research in operant and classical conditioning) • Exposure therapy • Habituation • Jerzy Konorski • Learned industriousness • Matching law • Negative (positive) contrast effect • Radical behaviorism (B.F. Skinner's philosophy) • Reinforcement learning • Reward system • Power and control in abusive relationships • Preference tests (animals) • Premack principle • Psychological manipulation • Sensitization • Social conditioning • Spontaneous recovery • Traumatic bonding 1.10.12 References [1] Domjan, Michael, Ed., The Principles of Learning and Behavior, Fifth Edition, Belmont, CA: Thomson/Wadsworth, 2003 [2] Thorndike, E.L. (1901). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in animals. Psychological Review Monograph Supplement, 2, 1–109. [3] Miltenberger, R. G.“Behavioral Modification: Principles and Procedures”. Thomson/Wadsworth, 2008. p. 9. [4] Skinner, B. F. “The Behavior of Organisms:An Experimental Analysis”, 1938 New York: Appleton-CenturyCrofts 41 [5] Skinner, B. F.“Are theories of learning necessary?" 1950, Psychological Review,57, 193-216. [6] Schacter, Daniel L., Daniel T. Gilbert, and Daniel M. Wegner. “B. F. Skinner: The role of reinforcement and Punishment”, subsection in: Psychology; Second Edition. New York: Worth, Incorporated, 2011, 278-288. [7] Ferster, C. B. & Skinner, B. F.“Schedules of Reinforcement”, 1957 New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts [8] Staddon, J. E. R; D. T Cerutti (February 2003).“Operant Conditioning”. Annual Review of Psychology 54 (1): 115. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145124. Retrieved 23 March 2013. [9] Mecca Chiesa (2004) Radical Behaviorism: The philosophy and the science [10] Skinner, B. F. “Science and Human Behavior”, 1953. New York: MacMillan [11] Skinner, B. F. “Verbal Behavior”, 1957. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts [12] Tucker, M., Sigafoos, J., & Bushell, H. (1998). Use of noncontingent reinforcement in the treatment of challenging behavior. Behavior Modification, 22, 529–547. [13] Poling, A., & Normand, M. (1999). Noncontingent reinforcement: an inappropriate description of time-based schedules that reduce behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 237–238. [14] Schacter et al.2011 Psychology 2nd ed. pg.280-284 Reference for entire section Principles version 130317 [15] Miltenberger, R. G.“Behavioral Modification: Principles and Procedures”. Thomson/Wadsworth, 2008. p. 84. [16] Miltenberger, R. G.“Behavioral Modification: Principles and Procedures”. Thomson/Wadsworth, 2008. p. 86. [17] Schultz, Wolfram (1998). Predictive Reward Signal of Dopamine Neurons. The Journal of Neurophysiology, 80(1), 1–27. [18] Neuringer, A. (2002). Operant variability: Evidence, functions, and theory. Psychonometric Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 672–705. [19] Pierce & Cheney (2004) Behavior Analysis and Learning [20] Cole, M.R. (1990). Operant hoarding: A new paradigm for the study of self-control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 247–262. [21] “Activity of pallidal neurons during movement”, M.R. DeLong, J. Neurophysiol., 34:414–27, 1971 [22] Richardson RT, DeLong MR (1991): Electrophysiological studies of the function of the nucleus basalis in primates. In Napier TC, Kalivas P, Hamin I (eds), The Basal Forebrain: Anatomy to Function (Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology, vol. 295. New York, Plenum, pp. 232–252 [23] PNAS 93:11219-24 1996, Science 279:1714–8 1998 42 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES [24] Neuron 63:244–253, 2009, Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 3: Article 13, 2009 • scienceofbehavior.com [25] Michael J. Frank, Lauren C. Seeberger, and Randall C. O'Reilly (2004) “By Carrot or by Stick: Cognitive Reinforcement Learning in Parkinsonism,”Science 4, November 2004 • An Introduction to Verbal Behavior Online Tutorial [26] Domjan, M. (2009). The Principles of Learning and Behavior. Wadsworth Publishing Company. 6th Edition. pages 244-249. [27] Timberlake, W. (1983). Rats' responses to a moving object related to food or water: A behavior-systems analysis. Animal Learning & Behavior. 11(3):309–320. [28] Neuringer, A.J. (1969). Animals respond for food in the presence of free food. Science. 166:399-401. • Society for Quantitative Analysis of Behavior • An Introduction to Relational Frame Theory Online Tutorial • [www.usu.edu/teachall/text/behavior/LRBIpdfs/ Positive.pdf] • [www.cehd.umn.edu/ceed/publications/tipsheets/ preschoolbehaviortipsheets/posrein.pdf] 1.11 Punishment (psychology) [29] Williams, D.R. and Williams, H. (1969). Automaintenance in the pigeon: sustained pecking despite contingent non-reinforcement. J. Exper. Analys. of Behav. 12:511–520. [30] Peden, B.F., Brown, M.P., & Hearst, E. (1977). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 3(4):377–399. [31] Gardner, R.A., & Gardner, B.T. (1988). Feedforward vs feedbackward: An ethological alternative to the law of effect. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 11:429–447. [32] Gardner, R. A. & Gardner B.T.(1998) The structure of learning from sign stimuli to sign language. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [33] Baum, W. M. (2012) Rethinking reinforcement: Allocation, induction and contingency. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 97, 101-124 . [34] Locurto, C. M., Terrace, H. S., & Gibbon, J. (1981) Autoshaping and conditioning theory. New York: Academic Press. 1. Staddon, J. E. R. & Cerutti, D. T. (2003) Operant behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 54:115-14 2. Kalat, J. (2013). Introduction to Psychology (10th ed.). Cengage Learning. 3. Elmes, D. (2011). Research Methods in Psychology (9th ed.). Cengage Learning. 4. Boyd, D. (2014). Lifespan Development (7th ed.). Cengage Learning. 5. Myers, D. (2011). Psychology (10th ed.). Cengage Learning. 6. Ormrod, J. (2011). Human Learning (6th ed.). Pearson. 7. Skinner, B.F. (1953). Science and Human Behavior. New York: Macmillan. 1.10.13 External links • Operant conditioning article in Scholarpedia • Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis • Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior • Negative reinforcement Diagram of operant conditioning In operant conditioning, punishment is any change in a human or animal's surroundings that occurs after a given behavior or response which reduces the likelihood of that behavior occurring again in the future. As with reinforcement, it is the behavior, not the animal, that is punished. Whether a change is or is not punishing is only known by its effect on the rate of the behavior, not by any “hostile”or aversive features of the change. For example, painful stimulation which would serve as a punisher in many cases serves to reinforce some behaviors of the masochist. 1.11.1 Types There are two types of punishment in operant conditioning: • positive punishment, punishment by application, or type I punishment, an experimenter punishes a response by presenting an aversive stimulus into the animal's surroundings (a brief electric shock, for example). 1.11. PUNISHMENT (PSYCHOLOGY) 43 • negative punishment, punishment by removal, or type II punishment, a valued, appetitive stimulus is removed (as in the removal of a feeding dish). As with reinforcement, it is not usually necessary to speak of positive and negative in regard to punishment. behavior and a punishing effect, the less effective the punishment will be. One major problem with a time delay between a behavior and a punishment is that other behaviors may present during that time delay. The subject may then associate the punishment given with the unintended behaviors, and thus suppressing those behaviors instead of the targeted behavior. Therefore, immediate Punishment is not a mirror effect of reinforcement. In ex- punishment is more effective in reducing a targeted beperiments with laboratory animals and studies with chil- havior than a delayed punishment would be. dren, punishment decreases the likelihood of a previously reinforced response only temporarily, and it can produce 1.11.4 Applied behavior analysis other “emotional”behavior (wing-flapping in pigeons, for example) and physiological changes (increased heart Main article: Applied behavior analysis rate, for example) that have no clear equivalents in reinforcement. Punishment is sometimes used for treatment programs in Punishment is considered by some behavioral psycholapplied behavior analysis in the most extreme cases, to ogists to be a “primary process”– a completely inreduce dangerous behaviors such as head banging or bitdependent phenomenon of learning, distinct from reining exhibited most commonly by children or people with forcement. Others see it as a category of negative reinspecial needs or disabilities. Punishment is considered forcement, creating a situation in which any punishmentone of the ethical challenges to autism treatment and is avoiding behavior (even standing still) is reinforced. one of the major reasons for discussion of professionalizing behavior analysis. Professionalizing behavior analysis through licensure would create a board to ensure that 1.11.2 Aversives consumers or families had a place to air disputes. (see Aversive stimulus, punisher, and punishing stimulus are Professional practice of behavior analysis) somewhat synonymous. Punishment may be used for (a) an aversive stimulus or (b) the occurrence of any 1.11.5 See also punishing change or (c) the part of an experiment in which a particular response is punished. However, some • Child grooming things considered aversive (such as spanking) can become • Emotional blackmail reinforcing. In addition, some things that are aversive may not be punishing if accompanying changes are re• Lovaas technique inforcing. A classic example would be mis-behavior that is 'punished' by a teacher but actually increases over time • Power and control in abusive relationships due to the reinforcing effects of attention on the student. • Psychological manipulation 1.11.3 Importance of contingency and contiguity • Punishment • Traumatic bonding One variable affecting punishment is contingency, which is defined as the dependency of events. A behavior may 1.11.6 References be dependent on a stimulus or dependent on a response. • Skinner, B. F. (1938) The behavior of organisms. The purpose of punishment is to reduce a behavior, and New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. the degree to which punishment is effective in reducing a targeted behavior is dependent on the relationship be• Chance, Paul. (2003) Learning and Behavior. 5th tween the behavior and a punishment. For example, if edition Toronto: Thomson-Wadsworth. a rat receives an aversive stimulus, such as a shock each • Holth, P. (2005). Two Definitions of Punishment. time it presses a lever, then it is clear that contingency The Behavior Analyst Today, 6(1), 43- 55 BAO . occurs between lever pressing and shock. In this case, the punisher (shock) is contingent upon the appearance of • http://www.class.uidaho.edu/psyc390/pdf/ the behavior (lever pressing). Punishment is most effec4-8-Side-Effects-and-Problems-with-Punishment. tive when contingency is present between a behavior and pdf a punisher. A second variable affecting punishment is contiguity, which is the closeness of events in time and/or • Chance, Paul. (2009)“Learning and Behavior: Acspace. Contiguity is important to reducing behavior betive Learning Edition.”6th edition Belmont, CA: cause the longer the time interval between an unwanted Wadsorth/Cengage Learning. 44 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES 1.12 Reinforcement ample is that many people can explain in detail where they were when they found out the World Trade Center * * This article is about the psychological concept. For was attacked. [3] [4] the construction materials reinforcement, see Rebar. Reinforcement is an important part of operant or For reinforcement learning in computer science, see instrumental conditioning. Reinforcement learning. For beam stiffening, see Stiffening. In behavioral psychology, reinforcement is a 1.12.1 Introduction B.F. Skinner was a high profile researcher that articulated many of the theoretical constructs of reinforcement and behaviorism. Skinner defined reinforcers according to the change in response strength rather than to more subjective criteria, such as what is pleasurable or valuable to someone. Accordingly, activities, foods or items considered pleasant or enjoyable may not necessarily be reinforcing (because they produce no increase in the response preceding them). Stimuli, settings, and activities only fit the definition of reinforcers if the behavior that immediately precedes the potential reinforcer increases in similar situations in the future, for example, a child who receives a cookie when he or she asks for one. If the frequency of “cookie-requesting behavior”increases, the cookie can be seen as reinforcing“cookie-requesting behavior”. If however,“cookie-requesting behavior”does not increase the cookie cannot be considered reinforcing. Diagram of operant conditioning The sole criteria that determines if an item, activity, or food is reinforcing is the change in probability of a behavior after administration of that potential reinforcer. Other theories may focus on additional factors such as whether the person expected the strategy to work at some point, but in the behavioral theory, reinforcement is descriptive of an increased probability of a response. consequence that will strengthen an organism's future behavior whenever that behavior is preceded by a specific antecedent stimulus. This strengthening effect may be measured as a higher frequency of behavior (e.g., pulling a lever more frequently), longer duration (e.g., pulling a lever for longer periods of time), greater magnitude (e.g., pulling a lever with greater force), or shorter la- The study of reinforcement has produced an enormous tency (e.g., pulling a lever more quickly following the an- body of reproducible experimental results. Reinforcement is the central concept and procedure in special edtecedent stimulus). ucation, applied behavior analysis, and the experimental Although in many cases a reinforcing stimulus is a re- analysis of behavior. warding stimulus which is “valued”or “liked”by the individual (e.g., money received from a slot machine, the taste of the treat, the euphoria produced by an addictive 1.12.2 Brief history drug), this is not a requirement. Indeed, reinforcement does not even require an individual to consciously per- Much of the work regarding reinforcement began with ceive an effect elicited by the stimulus.* [1] Furthermore, behavioral psychologists such as Edward Thorndike, J. stimuli that are “rewarding”or “liked”are not always B. Watson and B.F. Skinner and their use of animal exreinforcing: if an individual eats at a fast food restau- periments. B.F. Skinner is famous for his work on rerant (response) and likes the taste of the food (stimulus), inforcement and believed that positive reinforcement is but believes it is bad for their health, they may not eat superior to punishment in shaping behavior.* [8] At first it again and thus it was not reinforcing in that condition. glance, punishment can seem like just the opposite of Thus, reinforcement occurs only if there is an observable reinforcement, yet Skinner argued that they differ imstrengthening in behavior. mensely; he claimed that positive reinforcement results in In most cases reinforcement refers to an enhancement of behavior but this term may also refer to an enhancement of memory. One example of this effect is called posttraining reinforcement where a stimulus (e.g. food) given shortly after a training session enhances the learning.* [2] This stimulus can also be an emotional one. A good ex- lasting behavioral modification (long-term) whereas punishment changes behavior only temporarily (short-term) and has many detrimental side-effects. Skinner defined reinforcement as creating situations that a person likes or removing a situation he doesn't like, and punishment as removing a situation a person likes or setting up one he 1.12. REINFORCEMENT 45 doesn't like.* [8] Thus, the distinction was based mainly Negative reinforcement occurs when the rate of a beon the pleasant or aversive (unpleasant) nature of the havior increases because an aversive event or stimulus is stimulus. removed or prevented from happening.* [12]* :253 Two other researchers, Azrin and Holz, expanded upon operant conditioning by focusing on the definition of punishment in their chapter to Honigʼs volume on operant behavior, and they defined it as a“consequence of behavior that reduces the future probability of that behavior.” * [9] Skinnerʼs assumptions regarding reinforcement and punishment were thus challenged throughout the 1960s, and some studies have shown that positive reinforcement and punishment are equally effective in modifying behavior; that debate, however, continues in studies today as to whether or not reinforcement is more or equally as effective as punishment.* [10] Edward Thorndike also did some work regarding reinforcement in learning theory and believed that learning could occur unconsciously; that is, reinforcements or punishments could have an effect upon learning even if the person or organism is unaware of it.* [11] The research on the effects of positive and negative reinforcement alongside punishment continue today as those concepts apply directly to many forms of learning and behavior. • Example: A child cleans his or her room, and this behavior is followed by the parent stopping “nagging”or asking the child repeatedly to do so. Here, the nagging serves to negatively reinforce the behavior of cleaning because the child wants to remove that aversive stimulus of nagging. • Example: A person puts ointment on a bug bite to soothe an itch. If the ointment works, the person will likely increase the usage of the ointment because it resulted in removing the itch, which is the negative reinforcer. • Example: A company has a policy that if an employee completes their assigned work by Friday, they can have Saturday off. Working Saturday is the negative reinforcer, the employeeʼ s productivity will be increased as they avoid experiencing the negative reinforcer. Punishment 1.12.3 Operant conditioning Main article: Operant conditioning The basic definition is that a positive reinforcer adds a stimulus to increase or maintain frequency of a behavior while a negative reinforcer removes a stimulus to increase or maintain the frequency of the behavior. As mentioned above, positive and negative reinforcement are components of operant conditioning, along with positive punishment and negative punishment, all explained below: Reinforcement Positive reinforcement occurs when an event or stimulus is presented as a consequence of a behavior and the behavior increases.* [12]* :253 • Example: Whenever a rat presses a button, it gets a treat. If the rat starts pressing the button more often, the treat serves to positively reinforce this behavior. Positive punishment occurs when a response produces a stimulus and that responses decreases in probability in the future in similar circumstances. • Example: A mother yells at a child when he or she runs into the street. If the child stops running into the street, the yelling ceases. The yelling acts as positive punishment because the mother presents (adds) an unpleasant stimulus in the form of yelling. Negative punishment occurs when a response produces the removal of a stimulus and that response decreases in probability in the future in similar circumstances. • Example: A teenager comes home after curfew and the parents take away a privilege, such as cell phone usage. If the frequency of the child coming home late decreases, the privilege is gradually restored. The removal of the phone is negative punishment because the parents are taking away a pleasant stimulus (the phone) and motivating the child to return home earlier. • Example: A father gives candy to his daughter when Simply put, reinforcers serve to increase behaviors she picks up her toys. If the frequency of picking up whereas punishers serve to decrease behaviors; thus, posthe toys increases, the candy is a positive reinforcer itive reinforcers are stimuli that the subject will work to (to reinforce the behavior of cleaning up). attain, and negative reinforcers are stimuli that the subject * • Example: A company enacts a rewards program in will work to be rid of or to end. [13] The table below ilwhich employees earn prizes dependent on the num- lustrates the adding and subtracting of stimuli (pleasant ber of items sold. The prizes the employees receive or aversive) in relation to reinforcement vs. punishment. are the positive reinforcement as they increase sales. Further ideas and concepts: 46 • Distinguishing between positive and negative can be difficult and may not always be necessary; focusing on what is being removed or added and how it is being removed or added will determine the nature of the reinforcement. • Negative reinforcement is not punishment. The two, as explained above, differ in the increase (negative reinforcement) or decrease (punishment) of the future probability of a response. However, in negative reinforcement, the stimulus is an aversive stimulus, which if presented contingent on a response, may also function as a positive punisher. • The increase in behavior is independent of (i.e. not related to) whether or not the organism finds the reinforcer to be pleasant or aversive. Example: A child is given detention for acting up in school, but the frequency of the bad behavior increases. Thus, the detention is a reinforcer (could be positive or negative) even if the detention is not a pleasant stimuli, perhaps because the child now feels like a “rebel”or sees it as an opportunity to get out of class. CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES sleep, food, air, water, and sex. Some primary reinforcers, such as certain drugs, may mimic the effects of other primary reinforcers. While these primary reinforcers are fairly stable through life and across individuals, the reinforcing value of different primary reinforcers varies due to multiple factors (e.g., genetics, experience). Thus, one person may prefer one type of food while another abhors it. Or one person may eat lots of food while another eats very little. So even though food is a primary reinforcer for both individuals, the value of food as a reinforcer differs between them. Secondary reinforcers A secondary reinforcer, sometimes called a conditioned reinforcer, is a stimulus or situation that has acquired its function as a reinforcer after pairing with a stimulus that functions as a reinforcer. This stimulus may be a primary reinforcer or another conditioned reinforcer (such as money). An example of a secondary reinforcer would be the sound from a clicker, as used in clicker training. The sound of the clicker has been associated with praise or treats, and subsequently, the sound of the clicker may function as a reinforcer. As with primary reinforcers, an organism can experience satiation and deprivation with secondary reinforcers. • Some reinforcement can be simultaneously positive and negative, such as a drug addict taking drugs for the added euphoria (a positive feeling) and eliminating withdrawal symptoms (which would be a negative feeling). Or, in a warm room, a current of external air serves as positive reinforcement because it is pleasantly cool and as negative reinforcement Other reinforcement terms because it removes uncomfortable hot air. • A generalized reinforcer is a conditioned reinforcer • Reinforcement in the business world is essential in that has obtained the reinforcing function by pairdriving productivity. Employees are constantly moing with many other reinforcers and functions as a tivated by the ability to receive a positive stimulus, reinforcer under a wide-variety of motivating opersuch as a promotion or a bonus. Employees are also ations. (One example of this is money because it is driven by negative reinforcement. This can be seen paired with many other reinforcers).* [16]* :83 when employees are offered Saturdays off if they complete the weekly workload by Friday. • In reinforcer sampling, a potentially reinforcing but unfamiliar stimulus is presented to an organism • Though negative reinforcement has a positive effect without regard to any prior behavior. in the short term for a workplace (i.e. encourages a financially beneficial action), over-reliance on a neg• Socially-mediated reinforcement (direct reinforceative reinforcement hinders the ability of workers to ment) involves the delivery of reinforcement that react in a creative, engaged way creating growth in the quires the behavior of another organism. * long term. [14] • Both positive and negative reinforcement increase behavior. Most people, especially children, will learn to follow instruction by a mix of positive and negative reinforcement.* [12] Primary reinforcers A primary reinforcer, sometimes called an unconditioned reinforcer, is a stimulus that does not require pairing to function as a reinforcer and most likely has obtained this function through the evolution and its role in species' survival.* [15] Examples of primary reinforcers include • The Premack principle is a special case of reinforcement elaborated by David Premack, which states that a highly preferred activity can be used effectively as a reinforcer for a less-preferred activity.* [16]* :123 • Reinforcement hierarchy is a list of actions, rankordering the most desirable to least desirable consequences that may serve as a reinforcer. A reinforcement hierarchy can be used to determine the relative frequency and desirability of different activities, and is often employed when applying the Premack principle. 1.12. REINFORCEMENT • Contingent outcomes are more likely to reinforce behavior than non-contingent responses. Contingent outcomes are those directly linked to a causal behavior, such a light turning on being contingent on flipping a switch. Note that contingent outcomes are not necessary to demonstrate reinforcement, but perceived contingency may increase learning. 47 As can be seen from the above, artificial reinforcement is in fact created to build or develop skills, and to generalize, it is important that either a behavior trap is introduced to “capture”the skill and utilize naturally occurring reinforcement to maintain or increase it. This behavior trap may simply be a social situation that will generally result from a specific behavior once it has met a certain criterion (e.g., if you use edible reinforcers to train a person to say hello and smile at people when they meet them, after that skill has been built up, the natural reinforcer of other people smiling, and having more friendly interactions will naturally reinforce the skill and the edibles can be faded). • Contiguous stimuli are stimuli closely associated by time and space with specific behaviors. They reduce the amount of time needed to learn a behavior while increasing its resistance to extinction. Giving a dog a piece of food immediately after sitting is more contiguous with (and therefore more likely to reinforce) the behavior than a several minute delay in food de1.12.5 livery following the behavior. • Noncontingent reinforcement refers to responseindependent delivery of stimuli identified as reinforcers for some behaviors of that organism. However, this typically entails time-based delivery of stimuli identified as maintaining aberrant behavior, which decreases the rate of the target behavior.* [17] As no measured behavior is identified as being strengthened, there is controversy surrounding the use of the term noncontingent “reinforcement” .* [18] 1.12.4 Natural and artificial In his 1967 paper, Arbitrary and Natural Reinforcement, Charles Ferster proposed classifying reinforcement into events that increase frequency of an operant as a natural consequence of the behavior itself, and events that are presumed to affect frequency by their requirement of human mediation, such as in a token economy where subjects are “rewarded”for certain behavior with an arbitrary token of a negotiable value. In 1970, Baer and Wolf created a name for the use of natural reinforcers called “behavior traps”.* [19] A behavior trap requires only a simple response to enter the trap, yet once entered, the trap cannot be resisted in creating general behavior change. It is the use of a behavioral trap that increases a person's repertoire, by exposing them to the naturally occurring reinforcement of that behavior. Behavior traps have four characteristics: • They are “baited”with virtually irresistible reinforcers that “lure”the student to the trap • Only a low-effort response already in the repertoire is necessary to enter the trap • Interrelated contingencies of reinforcement inside the trap motivate the person to acquire, extend, and maintain targeted academic/social skills* [20] • They can remain effective for long periods of time because the person shows few, if any, satiation effects Intermittent reinforcement Pigeons experimented on in a scientific study were more responsive to intermittent reinforcement, than continuous reinforcement.* [21] In other words, pigeons were more prone to act when they only sometimes could get what they wanted. This effect was such that behavioral responses were maximized when the reward rate was at 50% (in other words, when the uncertainty was maximized), and would gradually decline toward values on either side of 50%.* [22] R.B Sparkman, a journalist specialized on what motivates human behavior, claims this is also true for humans, and may in part explain human tendencies such as gambling addiction.* [23] 1.12.6 Schedules When an animal's surroundings are controlled, its behavior patterns after reinforcement become predictable, even for very complex behavior patterns. A schedule of reinforcement is a rule or program that determines how and when the occurrence of a response will be followed by the delivery of the reinforcer, and extinction, in which no response is reinforced. Schedules of reinforcement influence how an instrumental response is learned and how it is maintained by reinforcement. Between these extremes is intermittent or partial reinforcement where only some responses are reinforced. Specific variations of intermittent reinforcement reliably induce specific patterns of response, irrespective of the species being investigated (including humans in some conditions). The orderliness and predictability of behavior under schedules of reinforcement was evidence for B.F. Skinner's claim that by using operant conditioning he could obtain “control over behavior”, in a way that rendered the theoretical disputes of contemporary comparative psychology obsolete. The reliability of schedule control supported the idea that a radical behaviorist experimental analysis of behavior could be the foundation for a psychology that did not refer to mental or cognitive processes. The reliability of schedules also led to the development of applied behavior analysis as a means of controlling or altering behavior. 48 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES Many of the simpler possibilities, and some of the more complex ones, were investigated at great length by Skinner using pigeons, but new schedules continue to be defined and investigated. Simple schedules • Variable ratio schedule (VR) – reinforced on average every nth response, but not always on the nth response.* [16]* :88 • Lab example: VR4”= first pellet delivered on 2 bar presses, second pellet delivered on 6 bar presses, third pellet 4 bar presses (2 + 6 + 4 = 12; 12/3= 4 bar presses to receive pellet). • Real-world example: slot machines (because, though the probability of hitting the jackpot is constant, the number of lever presses needed to hit the jackpot is variable). • Fixed interval (FI) – reinforced after n amount of time. • Example: FI1”= reinforcement provided for the first response after 1 second. • Lab example: FI15”= rat's bar-pressing behavior is reinforced for the first bar press after 15 seconds passes since the last reinforcement. • Real world example: washing machine cycle. A chart demonstrating the different response rate of the four simple schedules of reinforcement, each hatch mark designates a reinforcer being given • Ratio schedule – the reinforcement depends only on the number of responses the organism has performed. • Continuous reinforcement (CRF) – a schedule of reinforcement in which every occurrence of the instrumental response (desired response) is followed by the reinforcer.* [16]* :86 • Lab example: each time a rat presses a bar it gets a pellet of food. • Real world example: each time a dog defecates outside its owner gives it a treat; each time a person puts $1 in a candy machine and presses the buttons he receives a candy bar. • Variable interval (VI) – reinforced on an average of n amount of time, but not always exactly n amount of time.* [16]* :89 • Example: VI4”= first pellet delivered after 2 minutes, second delivered after 6 minutes, third is delivered after 4 minutes (2 + 6 + 4 = 12; 12/ 3 = 4). Reinforcement is delivered on the average after 4 minutes. • Lab example: VI10”= a rat's bar-pressing behavior is reinforced for the first bar press after an average of 10 seconds passes since the last reinforcement. • Real world example: checking your e-mail or pop quizzes. Going fishing̶you might catch a fish after 10 minutes, then have to wait an hour, then have to wait 18 minutes. Other simple schedules include: Simple schedules have a single rule to determine when a single type of reinforcer is delivered for specific response. • Fixed ratio (FR) – schedules deliver reinforcement after every nth response.* [16]* :88 • Example: FR2”= every second desired response the subject makes is reinforced. • Lab example: FR5”= rat's bar-pressing behavior is reinforced with food after every 5 barpresses in a Skinner box. • Real-world example: FR10”= Used car dealer gets a $1000 bonus for each 10 cars sold on the lot. • Differential reinforcement of incompatible behavior – Used to reduce a frequent behavior without punishing it by reinforcing an incompatible response. An example would be reinforcing clapping to reduce nose picking. • Differential reinforcement of other behavior (DRO) – Also known as omission training procedures, an instrumental conditioning procedure in which a positive reinforcer is periodically delivered only if the participant does something other than the target response. An example would be reinforcing any hand action other than nose picking.* [16]* :338 1.12. REINFORCEMENT • Differential reinforcement of low response rate (DRL) – Used to encourage low rates of responding. It is like an interval schedule, except that premature responses reset the time required between behavior. • Lab example: DRL10”= a rat is reinforced for the first response after 10 seconds, but if the rat responds earlier than 10 seconds there is no reinforcement and the rat has to wait 10 seconds from that premature response without another response before bar pressing will lead to reinforcement. • Real world example: “If you ask me for a potato chip no more than once every 10 minutes, I will give it to you. If you ask more often, I will give you none.” • Differential reinforcement of high rate (DRH) – Used to increase high rates of responding. It is like an interval schedule, except that a minimum number of responses are required in the interval in order to receive reinforcement. • Lab example: DRH10"/15 responses = a rat must press a bar 15 times within a 10 second increment to get reinforced. • Real world example: “If Lance Armstrong is going to win the Tour de France he has to pedal x number of times during the y-hour race.” • Fixed time (FT) – Provides reinforcement at a fixed time since the last reinforcement, irrespective of whether the subject has responded or not. In other words, it is a non-contingent schedule. • Lab example: FT5”= rat gets food every 5 seconds regardless of the behavior. • Real world example: a person gets an annuity check every month regardless of behavior between checks • Variable time (VT) – Provides reinforcement at an average variable time since last reinforcement, regardless of whether the subject has responded or not. Effects of different types of simple schedules • Fixed ratio: activity slows after reinforcer and then picks up. • Variable ratio: high rate of responding, greatest activity of all schedules, responding rate is high and stable. 49 • Ratio schedules produce higher rates of responding than interval schedules, when the rates of reinforcement are otherwise similar. • Variable schedules produce higher rates and greater resistance to extinction than most fixed schedules. This is also known as the Partial Reinforcement Extinction Effect (PREE). • The variable ratio schedule produces both the highest rate of responding and the greatest resistance to extinction (for example, the behavior of gamblers at slot machines). • Fixed schedules produce “post-reinforcement pauses”(PRP), where responses will briefly cease immediately following reinforcement, though the pause is a function of the upcoming response requirement rather than the prior reinforcement.* [24] • The PRP of a fixed interval schedule is frequently followed by a “scallop-shaped”accelerating rate of response, while fixed ratio schedules produce a more“angular”response. • fixed interval scallop: the pattern of responding that develops with fixed interval reinforcement schedule, performance on a fixed interval reflects subject's accuracy in telling time. • Organisms whose schedules of reinforcement are “thinned”(that is, requiring more responses or a greater wait before reinforcement) may experience “ratio strain”if thinned too quickly. This produces behavior similar to that seen during extinction. • Ratio strain: the disruption of responding that occurs when a fixed ratio response requirement is increased too rapidly. • Ratio run: high and steady rate of responding that completes each ratio requirement. Usually higher ratio requirement causes longer post-reinforcement pauses to occur. • Partial reinforcement schedules are more resistant to extinction than continuous reinforcement schedules. • Ratio schedules are more resistant than interval schedules and variable schedules more resistant than fixed ones. • Momentary changes in reinforcement value lead to dynamic changes in behavior.* [25] Compound schedules • Fixed interval: activity increases as deadline nears, Compound schedules combine two or more different simcan cause fast extinction. ple schedules in some way using the same reinforcer for • Variable interval: steady activity results, good resis- the same behavior. There are many possibilities; among tance to extinction. those most often used are: 50 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES • Alternative schedules – A type of compound schedule where two or more simple schedules are in effect and whichever schedule is completed first results in reinforcement.* [26] • Conjunctive schedules – A complex schedule of reinforcement where two or more simple schedules are in effect independently of each other, and requirements on all of the simple schedules must be met for reinforcement. • Multiple schedules – Two or more schedules alternate over time, with a stimulus indicating which is in force. Reinforcement is delivered if the response requirement is met while a schedule is in effect. • Example: FR4 when given a whistle and FI6 when given a bell ring. • Tandem schedules – Reinforcement occurs when two or more successive schedule requirements have been completed, with no stimulus indicating when a schedule has been completed and the next has started. • Example: VR10, after it is completed the schedule is changed without warning to FR10, after that it is changed without warning to FR16, etc. At the end of the series of schedules, a reinforcer is finally given. • Higher-order schedules – completion of one schedule is reinforced according to a second schedule; e.g. in FR2 (FI10 secs), two successive fixed interval schedules require completion before a response is reinforced. • Mixed schedules – Either of two, or more, schedules may occur with no stimulus indicating which is Superimposed schedules in force. Reinforcement is delivered if the response requirement is met while a schedule is in effect. The psychology term superimposed schedules of rein• Example: FI6 and then VR3 without any stim- forcement refers to a structure of rewards where two or more simple schedules of reinforcement operate simultaulus warning of the change in schedule. neously. Reinforcers can be positive, negative, or both. • Concurrent schedules – A complex reinforcement An example is a person who comes home after a long procedure in which the participant can choose any day at work. The behavior of opening the front door is reone of two or more simple reinforcement sched- warded by a big kiss on the lips by the person's spouse and ules that are available simultaneously. Organisms a rip in the pants from the family dog jumping enthusiasare free to change back and forth between the re- tically. Another example of superimposed schedules of sponse alternatives at any time. reinforcement is a pigeon in an experimental cage pecking at a button. The pecks deliver a hopper of grain every • Real world example: changing channels on a 20th peck, and access to water after every 200 pecks. television. Superimposed schedules of reinforcement are a type of • Concurrent-chain schedule of reinforcement – compound schedule that evolved from the initial work on A complex reinforcement procedure in which the simple schedules of reinforcement by B.F. Skinner and participant is permitted to choose during the first his colleagues (Skinner and Ferster, 1957). They demonlink which of several simple reinforcement sched- strated that reinforcers could be delivered on schedules, ules will be in effect in the second link. Once a and further that organisms behaved differently under difchoice has been made, the rejected alternatives be- ferent schedules. Rather than a reinforcer, such as food come unavailable until the start of the next trial. or water, being delivered every time as a consequence of • Interlocking schedules – A single schedule with some behavior, a reinforcer could be delivered after more two components where progress in one component than one instance of the behavior. For example, a pigeon affects progress in the other component. An inter- may be required to peck a button switch ten times belocking FR60–FI120, for example, each response fore food appears. This is a “ratio schedule”. Also, subtracts time from the interval component such that a reinforcer could be delivered after an interval of time each response is “equal”to removing two seconds passed following a target behavior. An example is a rat that is given a food pellet immediately following the first from the FI. response that occurs after two minutes has elapsed since • Chained schedules – Reinforcement occurs after the last lever press. This is called an “interval schedule” two or more successive schedules have been com- . pleted, with a stimulus indicating when one schedule In addition, ratio schedules can deliver reinforcement folhas been completed and the next has started lowing fixed or variable number of behaviors by the indi• Example: FR10 in a green light when com- vidual organism. Likewise, interval schedules can deliver pleted it goes to a yellow light to indicate FR3, reinforcement following fixed or variable intervals of time after it is completed it goes into red light to following a single response by the organism. Individual indicate VI6, etc. At the end of the chain, a behaviors tend to generate response rates that differ based reinforcer is given. upon how the reinforcement schedule is created. Much 1.12. REINFORCEMENT 51 subsequent research in many labs examined the effects pecking keys; pecking responses can be made on either, on behaviors of scheduling reinforcers. and food reinforcement might follow a peck on either. If an organism is offered the opportunity to choose be- The schedules of reinforcement arranged for pecks on the tween or among two or more simple schedules of re- two keys can be different. They may be independent, or inforcement at the same time, the reinforcement struc- they may be linked so that behavior on one key affects ture is called a “concurrent schedule of reinforcement” the likelihood of reinforcement on the other. . Brechner (1974, 1977) introduced the concept of superimposed schedules of reinforcement in an attempt to create a laboratory analogy of social traps, such as when humans overharvest their fisheries or tear down their rainforests. Brechner created a situation where simple reinforcement schedules were superimposed upon each other. In other words, a single response or group of responses by an organism led to multiple consequences. Concurrent schedules of reinforcement can be thought of as “or” schedules, and superimposed schedules of reinforcement can be thought of as“and”schedules. Brechner and Linder (1981) and Brechner (1987) expanded the concept to describe how superimposed schedules and the social trap analogy could be used to analyze the way energy flows through systems. It is not necessary for responses on the two schedules to be physically distinct. In an alternate way of arranging concurrent schedules, introduced by Findley in 1958, both schedules are arranged on a single key or other response device, and the subject can respond on a second key to change between the schedules. In such a “Findley concurrent”procedure, a stimulus (e.g., the color of the main key) signals which schedule is in effect. Concurrent schedules often induce rapid alternation between the keys. To prevent this, a“changeover delay”is commonly introduced: each schedule is inactivated for a brief period after the subject switches to it. When both the concurrent schedules are variable intervals, a quantitative relationship known as the matching law is found between relative response rates in the two schedules and the relative reinforcement rates they deliver; this was first observed by R.J. Herrnstein in 1961. Matching law is a rule for instrumental behavior which states that the relative rate of responding on a particular response alternative equals the relative rate of reinforcement for that response (rate of behavior = rate of reinforcement). Animals and humans have a tendency to prefer choice in schedules.* [27] Superimposed schedules of reinforcement have many real-world applications in addition to generating social traps. Many different human individual and social situations can be created by superimposing simple reinforcement schedules. For example a human being could have simultaneous tobacco and alcohol addictions. Even more complex situations can be created or simulated by superimposing two or more concurrent schedules. For example, a high school senior could have a choice between going to Stanford University or UCLA, and at the same time have the choice of going into the Army or the Air Force, 1.12.7 Shaping and simultaneously the choice of taking a job with an internet company or a job with a software company. That Main article: Shaping (psychology) is a reinforcement structure of three superimposed concurrent schedules of reinforcement. Shaping is reinforcement of successive approximations to Superimposed schedules of reinforcement can create a desired instrumental response. In training a rat to press the three classic conflict situations (approach–approach a lever, for example, simply turning toward the lever is conflict, approach–avoidance conflict, and avoidance– reinforced at first. Then, only turning and stepping toward avoidance conflict) described by Kurt Lewin (1935) and it is reinforced. The outcomes of one set of behaviours can operationalize other Lewinian situations analyzed by starts the shaping process for the next set of behaviours, his force field analysis. Other examples of the use of su- and the outcomes of that set prepares the shaping process perimposed schedules of reinforcement as an analytical for the next set, and so on. As training progresses, the tool are its application to the contingencies of rent control response reinforced becomes progressively more like the (Brechner, 2003) and problem of toxic waste dumping in desired behavior; each subsequent behaviour becomes a the Los Angeles County storm drain system (Brechner, closer approximation of the final behaviour.* [28] 2010). 1.12.8 Chaining Concurrent schedules In operant conditioning, concurrent schedules of reinforcement are schedules of reinforcement that are simultaneously available to an animal subject or human participant, so that the subject or participant can respond on either schedule. For example, in a two-alternative forced choice task, a pigeon in a Skinner box is faced with two Main article: Chaining Chaining involves linking discrete behaviors together in a series, such that each result of each behavior is both the reinforcement (or consequence) for the previous behavior, and the stimuli (or antecedent) for the next behavior. There are many ways to teach chaining, such as forward 52 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES chaining (starting from the first behavior in the chain), backwards chaining (starting from the last behavior) and total task chaining (in which the entire behavior is taught from beginning to end, rather than as a series of steps). An example is opening a locked door. First the key is inserted, then turned, then the door opened. In instrumental learning situations, which involve operant behavior, the persuasive communicator will present his message and then wait for the receiver to make a correct response. As soon as the receiver makes the response, the communicator will attempt to fix the response by some appropriate reward or reinforcement.* [30] Forward chaining would teach the subject first to insert the key. Once that task is mastered, they are told to insert the key, and taught to turn it. Once that task is mastered, they are told to perform the first two, then taught to open the door. Backwards chaining would involve the teacher first inserting and turning the key, and the subject is taught to open the door. Once that is learned, the teacher inserts the key, and the subject is taught to turn it, then opens the door as the next step. Finally, the subject is taught to insert the key, and they turn and open the door. Once the first step is mastered, the entire task has been taught. Total task chaining would involve teaching the entire task as a single series, prompting through all steps. Prompts are faded (reduced) at each step as they are mastered. In conditional learning situations, where there is respondent behavior, the communicator presents his message so as to elicit the response he wants from the receiver, and the stimulus that originally served to elicit the response then becomes the reinforcing or rewarding element in conditioning.* [29] 1.12.9 1.12.10 Mathematical models A lot of work has been done in building a mathematical model of reinforcement. This model is known as MPR, short for mathematical principles of reinforcement. Killeen and Sitomer are among the key researchers in this field. Persuasive communication & the reinforcement theory 1.12.11 Criticisms Persuasive communication Persuasion influences any person the way they think, act and feel. Persuasive skill tells about how people understand the concern, position and needs of the people. Persuasion can be classified into informal persuasion and formal persuasion. Informal persuasion This tells about the way in which a person interacts with his/her colleagues and customers. The informal persuasion can be used in team, memos as well as e-mails. Formal persuasion This type of persuasion is used in writing customer letter, proposal and also for formal presentation to any customer or colleagues. Process of persuasion Persuasion relates how you influence people with your skills, experience, knowledge, leadership, qualities and team capabilities. Persuasion is an interactive process while getting the work done by others. Here are examples for which you can use persuasion skills in real time. Interview: you can prove your best talents, skills and expertise. Clients: to guide your clients for the achievement of the goals or targets. Memos: to express your ideas and views to coworkers for the improvement in the operations. Resistance identification and positive attitude are the vital roles of persuasion. The standard definition of behavioral reinforcement has been criticized as circular, since it appears to argue that response strength is increased by reinforcement, and defines reinforcement as something that increases response strength (i.e., response strength is increased by things that increase response strength). However, the correct usage* [31] of reinforcement is that something is a reinforcer because of its effect on behavior, and not the other way around. It becomes circular if one says that a particular stimulus strengthens behavior because it is a reinforcer, and does not explain why a stimulus is producing that effect on the behavior. Other definitions have been proposed, such as F.D. Sheffield's“consummatory behavior contingent on a response”, but these are not broadly used in psychology.* [32] History of the terms In the 1920s Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov may have been the first to use the word reinforcement with respect to behavior, but (according to Dinsmoor) he used its approximate Russian cognate sparingly, and even then it referred to strengthening an already-learned but weakening response. He did not use it, as it is today, for selecting and strengthening new behaviors. Pavlov's introduction of the word extinction (in Russian) approximates today's Persuasion is a form of human interaction. It takes place psychological use. when one individual expects some particular response In popular use, positive reinforcement is often used as a from one or more other individuals and deliberately sets synonym for reward, with people (not behavior) thus beout to secure the response through the use of commu- ing“reinforced”, but this is contrary to the term's consisnication. The communicator must realize that different tent technical usage, as it is a dimension of behavior, and groups have different values.* [29]* :24–25 not the person, which is strengthened. Negative reinforce- 1.12. REINFORCEMENT ment is often used by laypeople and even social scientists outside psychology as a synonym for punishment. This is contrary to modern technical use, but it was B.F. Skinner who first used it this way in his 1938 book. By 1953, however, he followed others in thus employing the word punishment, and he re-cast negative reinforcement for the removal of aversive stimuli. There are some within the field of behavior analysis* [33] who have suggested that the terms“positive”and“negative”constitute an unnecessary distinction in discussing reinforcement as it is often unclear whether stimuli are being removed or presented. For example, Iwata poses the question: "...is a change in temperature more accurately characterized by the presentation of cold (heat) or the removal of heat (cold)?"* [34]* :363 Thus, reinforcement could be conceptualized as a pre-change condition replaced by a post-change condition that reinforces the behavior that followed the change in stimulus conditions. 1.12.12 Applications Partial or intermittent negative reinforcement can create an effective climate of fear and doubt.* [35] 1.12.13 See also • Applied behavior analysis • Behavioral cusp • Child grooming • Dog training • Learned industriousness • Overjustification effect • Power and control in abusive relationships • Psychological manipulation • Punishment • Reinforcement learning • Reward system • Society for Quantitative Analysis of Behavior • Traumatic bonding 1.12.14 References [1] Winkielman P., Berridge KC, and Wilbarger JL. (2005). Unconscious affective reactions to masked happy verses angry faces influence consumption behavior and judgement value. Pers Soc Psychol Bull: 31, 121–35. 53 [2] Mondadori C, Waser PG, and Huston JP. (2005). Timedependent effects of post-trial reinforcement, punishment or ECS on passive avoidance learning. Physiol Behav: 18, 1103–9. PMID 928533 [3] White NM, Gottfried JA (2011). “Reward: What Is It? How Can It Be Inferred from Behavior?". PMID 22593908. [4] White NM. (2011). Reward: What is it? How can it be inferred from behavior. In: Neurobiology of Sensation and Reward. CRC Press PMID 22593908 [5] Malenka RC, Nestler EJ, Hyman SE (2009). “Chapter 15: Reinforcement and Addictive Disorders”. In Sydor A, Brown RY. Molecular Neuropharmacology: A Foundation for Clinical Neuroscience (2nd ed.). New York: McGrawHill Medical. pp. 364–375. ISBN 9780071481274. [6] Nestler EJ (December 2013). “Cellular basis of memory for addiction”. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 15 (4): 431– 443. PMC 3898681. PMID 24459410. [7] “Glossary of Terms”. Mount Sinai School of Medicine. Department of Neuroscience. Retrieved 9 February 2015. [8] Skinner, B.F. (1948). Macmillan Company. Walden Two. Toronto: The [9] Honig, Werner (1966). Operant Behavior: Areas of Research and Application. New York: Meredith Publishing Company. p. 381. [10] Domjan, W. (2003). Aversive control: Avoidance and punishment. In: The Principles of Learning and Behavior. CA: Thompson Learning. p. 302. [11] Shanks, David (2010). “Learning: From Association to Cognition”. Annual Review of Psychology (61): 273–301. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100519. [12] Flora, Stephen (2004). The Power of Reinforcement. Albany: State University of New York Press. [13] D'Amato, M. R. (1969). Melvin H. Marx, ed. Learning Processes: Instrumental Conditioning. Toronto: The Macmillan Company. [14] Harter, J. K. (2002). C. L. Keyes, ed. Well-Being in the Workplace and its Relationship to Business Outcomes: A Review of the Gallup Studies. Washington D.C.: American Psychological Association. [15] Skinner, B.F. (1974). About Behaviorism [16] Miltenberger, R. G.“Behavioral Modification: Principles and Procedures”. Thomson/Wadsworth, 2008. [17] Tucker, M.; Sigafoos, J. & Bushell, H. (1998). Use of noncontingent reinforcement in the treatment of challenging behavior. Behavior Modification, 22, 529–47. [18] Poling, A. & Normand, M. (1999). Noncontingent reinforcement: an inappropriate description of time-based schedules that reduce behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 237–8. 54 [19] Baer and Wolf, 1970, The entry into natural communities of reinforcement. In R. Ulrich, T. Stachnik, & J. Mabry (eds.), Control of human behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 319–24). Gleenview, IL: Scott, Foresman. [20] Kohler & Greenwood, 1986, Toward a technology of generalization: The identification of natural contingencies of reinforcement. The Behavior Analyst, 9, 19–26. [21] http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/ PMC1333219/ [22] Zeiler, MD (March 1972). “Fixed-interval behavior: effects of percentage reinforcement.”. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 17 (2): 177–89. doi:10.1901/jeab.1972.17-177. PMID 16811580. [23] Sparkman, R. B. (1979). The Art of Manipulation. Doubleday Publishing. p. 34. ISBN 0385270070. [24] Derenne, A. & Flannery, K.A. (2007). Within Session FR Pausing. The Behavior Analyst Today, 8(2), 175–86 BAO [25] McSweeney, F.K.; Murphy, E.S. & Kowal, B.P. (2001) Dynamic Changes in Reinforcer Value: Some Misconceptions and Why You Should Care. The Behavior Analyst Today, 2(4), 341–7 BAO [26] Iversen, I.H. & Lattal, K.A. Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1991, Elsevier, Amsterdam. [27] Toby L. Martin, C.T. Yu, Garry L. Martin & Daniela Fazzio (2006): On Choice, Preference, and Preference For Choice. The Behavior Analyst Today, 7(2), 234–48 BAO [28] Schacter, Daniel L., Daniel T. Gilbert, and Daniel M. Wegner. “Chapter 7: Learning.”Psychology. ; Second Edition. N.p.: Worth, Incorporated, 2011. 284-85. [29] Bettinghaus, Erwin P., Persuasive Communication, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1968 CHAPTER 1. CATEGORIES 1.12.15 Further reading • Brechner, K.C. (1974) An experimental analysis of social traps. PhD Dissertation, Arizona State University. • Brechner, K.C. (1977). An experimental analysis of social traps. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 552–64. • Brechner, K.C. (1987) Social Traps, Individual Traps, and Theory in Social Psychology. Pasadena, CA: Time River Laboratory, Bulletin No. 870001. • Brechner, K.C. (2003) Superimposed schedules applied to rent control. Economic and Game Theory, 2/28/03, . • Brechner, K.C. (2010) A social trap analysis of the Los Angeles County storm drain system: A rational for interventions. Paper presented at the annual convention of the American Psychological Association, San Diego. • Brechner, K.C. & Linder, D.E. (1981), A social trap analysis of energy distribution systems, in Advances in Environmental Psychology, Vol. 3, Baum, A. & Singer, JE, eds. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum & Associates. • Chance, Paul. (2003) Learning and Behavior. 5th edition Toronto: Thomson-Wadsworth. • Dinsmoor, James A. (2004) "The etymology of basic concepts in the experimental analysis of behavior". Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 82(3): 311–6. [30] Skinner, B.F., The Behavior of Organisms. An Experimental Analysis, New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. 1938 • Ferster, C.B. & Skinner, B.F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-CenturyCrofts. ISBN 0-13-792309-0. [31] Epstein, L.H. 1982. Skinner for the Classroom. Champaign, IL: Research Press • Lewin, K. (1935) A dynamic theory of personality: Selected papers. New York: McGraw-Hill. [32] Franco J. Vaccarino, Bernard B. Schiff & Stephen E. Glickman (1989). Biological view of reinforcement. in Stephen B. Klein and Robert Mowrer. Contemporary learning theories: Instrumental conditioning theory and the impact of biological constraints on learning. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates • Michael, Jack. (1975) "Positive and negative reinforcement, a distinction that is no longer necessary; or a better way to talk about bad things". Behaviorism, 3(1): 33–44. [33] Michael, J. (1975, 2005). Positive and negative reinforcement, a distinction that is no longer necessary; or a better way to talk about bad things. Journal of Organizational Behavior Management, 24, 207–22. [34] Iwata, B.A. (1987). Negative reinforcement in applied behavior analysis: an emerging technology. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 361–78. [35] Braiker, Harriet B. (2004). Who's Pulling Your Strings ? How to Break The Cycle of Manipulation. ISBN 0-07144672-9. • Skinner, B.F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. • Skinner, B.F. (1956). A case history in scientific method. American Psychologist, 11, 221–33. • Zeiler, M.D. (1968) Fixed and variable schedules of response-independent reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 405–14. • Glossary of reinforcement terms at the University of Iowa 1.12. REINFORCEMENT • Harter, J.K., Shmidt, F.L., & Keyes, C.L. (2002). Well-Being in the Workplace and its Relationship to Business Outcomes: A Review of the Gallup Studies. In C.L. Keyes & J. Haidt (Eds.), Flourishing: The Positive Person and the Good Life (pp. 205– 224). Washington D.C.: American Psychological Association. 1.12.16 External links • An On-Line Positive Reinforcement Tutorial • Scholarpedia Reinforcement • scienceofbehavior.com 55 Chapter 2 ARTICLES 2.1 Alpha roll wild. Dr. Mech refers to this behavior as pinning, which he describes as a dominance behavior.* [8] These domAn alpha roll is a technique used in dog training to dis- inance behaviors are shown significantly more often by cipline a misbehaving dog. It consists of flipping the dog the breeding pair of the pack, but the purpose or role of onto its back and holding it in that position, sometimes the behavior is controversial. by the throat. The theory is that this teaches the dog that This suggests that this ritual does not serve as a behavthe trainer is the pack leader (or alpha animal). ioral correction or punishment, nor as a reinforcement of the dominance of the breeding pair. On the other hand, dyadic play between wolves involves behavior like pin2.1.1 History ning. Wolf puppy play patterns demonstrate that puppies prefer to assume the dominant role in play (see dog The alpha roll was first widely popularized by the Monks behavior), and avoid the submissive roles such as being of New Skete, in the 1978 book“How To Be Your Dog's pinned. This suggests that dogs do not instinctually panic Best Friend”.* [1] (In the 2002 second edition of the if they are forced into this submissive position against book, the monks recanted and strongly discouraged the their will. Although neither of these positions speaks ditechnique, describing it as“too risky and demanding for rectly to the issue of whether the alpha roll is an effective the average dog owner.”* [2]) Although the 1976 book correction tool, it does call into question the behavioral itself is widely regarded as a classic in dog training litera- validity of the technique. ture and highly recommended for people trying to better understand their dog, the alpha roll is now highly controversial among animal behaviorists, since the theory of 2.1.3 Contemporary use canine dominance has been drawn into question. In the original context, the alpha roll was only meant to be used Position statements on dominance released by AVSAB in the most serious cases.* [3] The theory behind the al- and APDT in 2009 draw into question the science bepha roll is based on a research study of captive wolves hind techniques that rely on dominance theory. It should kept in an area too small for their numbers and composed never be used by inexperienced trainers, and never to corof members that wouldn't be found together in a pack in rect undesired behavior caused by the dog's failure to unthe wild. These conditions resulted in increased numbers derstand your command. Used in a controlled way and of conflicts that scientists today know are not typical of coupled with praise and rewards when the dog changes wolves living in the wild.* [4] Behaviors seen in wolves its behavior appropriately, it may have positive effect, (specifically the alpha roll) living in atypical social groups but there is disagreement about its long-term effectiveand crowded conditions does not translate to dog training ness and safety. A 2009 study by University of Bristolʼs especially since using the technique can be harmful to the Department of Clinical Veterinary Sciences showed that methods of handling that relied on dominance theory achandler and the dog.* [5] tually provoked aggressive behavior in dogs with no previous known history of aggression. 2.1.2 Effects It has been argued by some that a dog will only forcibly 2.1.4 Further sources flip another animal onto its back during a serious fight • Melissa Alexander. “The History and Misconcepwhere the intent may be to kill the opponent* [6]* [7]. tions of Dominance Theory”. Archived from the Further, the name alpha roll is considered a misnomer original on 13 October 2007. Retrieved 8 October by top wolf experts, such as David Mech, because the 2007. practice as used as a behavioral correction bears little relation to the natural behavior shown by wolves in the • Carmen Buitrago. “Debunking the Dominance 56 2.2. ANIMAL ATTACKS Myth”. Retrieved 8 October 2007. • Kathy Diamond Davis. “Alpha-Roll Training Can Cause Serious Problems”. veterinarypartner.com. Archived from the original on 28 September 2007. Retrieved 8 October 2007. • Krista Mifflin. “About That Alpha Roll”. about.com. Archived from the original on 15 October 2007. Retrieved 8 October 2007. • Deb McKean. “Canine Handling in a Clinical Setting”. Archived from the original on 30 September 2007. Retrieved 8 October 2007. 57 [6] Nicole Wilde, CPDT (2001). “Leadership vs. Dominance”. Retrieved 8 October 2007. [7] Dr. Ian Dunbar. “History & Misconceptions of Dominance Theory”. Archived from the original on 27 September 2007. Retrieved 8 October 2007. [8] L. David Mech (1999). “Alpha status, dominance, and division of labor in wolf packs”(PDF). Canadian Journal of Zoology 77:1196-1203. Retrieved 8 October 2007. 2.2 Animal attacks Animal attacks are an uncommon cause of human fatal• Cindy Tittle Moore. “rec.pets.dogs Behaviour ities and injuries. The frequency of animal attacks varies FAQ”. Archived from the original on 29 September with geographical location and historical period. Attacks 2007. Retrieved 8 October 2007. described in the following article have occurred in historical times with documentation. Instances of attacks that • Mark Plonsky, PhD. “Punishment: Problems & can be attributed to the animal being confined or 'trapped' Principles for Effective Use”. Archived from the prior to an attack have not been included. Serious injuries original on 14 October 2007. Retrieved 8 October and fatalities are more likely to be incurred by infants, 2007. children and those with limited ability to defend themselves against an animal. A person is more likely to be • Kelly Ryan.“Are You the Alpha?". Archived from killed by an animal than they are to die from being hit by the original on 29 September 2007. Retrieved 8 Oclightening.* [1] tober 2007. Animal attacks have been identified as a major public • Terry Ryan (2001). “Assessing the Alpha Roll” health problem. In 1997 it was estimated that up to (PDF). The Association of Pet Dog Trainers. 2 million animal bites occur each year in the United Archived from the original on 29 September 2007. States. Injuries caused by animal attacks result in thouRetrieved 8 October 2007. sands of fatalities worldwide every year.* [2] All causes of death are reported to the Center for disease control each year. Medical injury codes are used to identify specific 2.1.5 Footnotes cases.* [3] The World Health Organization uses identical coding, though it is unclear whether all countries keep [1] Monks of New Skete, The (1978). How To Be Your Dog's track of fatalities caused by animals. Best Friend. Little, Brown & Company. ISBN 0-31660491-7. 2.2.1 Alligators [2] Monks of New Skete, The (2002). How To Be Your Dog's Best Friend. Little, Brown & Company. ISBN 0-31661000-3. In the original edition of this book, we recom- Main article: List of fatal alligator attacks in the United mended a technique we termed“the alpha-wolf rollover” States by decade ...We no longer recommend this technique and strongly discourage its use to our clients.... The conditions in which it might be used effectively are simply too risky and demanding for the average dog owner; there are other ways 2.2.2 Arthropods of dealing with problem behavior that are much safer and, in the long run, just as effective. Bees, Wasps, Scorpions, and other stinging or biting [4] Adam Miklosi, PhD (2007). Dog Behavior, Evolution and Cognition. Oxford University Press. pp. 83–93. ISBN 978-0-19-295852-5. arthropods cause fatalities but these are not as often characterized as 'attacks'. It may be difficult to characterize some of these encounters as offensive or defensive. An arthropod 'attack' instead of causing tissue trauma such as cutting, lacerating, crushing or the severing of body parts may instead cause a physiological reaction that results in a human death. These effects are toxic effects and allergic effects. [5] Sophia Yin, DVM, MS.“New Study Finds Popular“Alpha Dog”Training Techniques Can Cause More Harm Than Good”. Retrieved 5 May 2011. Listing deaths due stings and allergic reactions from arthropods is not practical but some of the more unusual cases include: [3] AVSAB. “Position Statement on the Use of Dominance Theory in Behavior Modification of Animals”. American Veterinary Society of Animal Behavior. Retrieved 4 May 2011. 58 CHAPTER 2. ARTICLES central Japan.* [32] Ants In 2006 a 68-year-old South Carolina woman died after being attacked by fire ants while gardening.* [4] Residents European brown bear in nursing homes have been attacked.* [5] Jumper jack ants have caused numerous fatalities. In Main article: Eurasian brown bear 1931 two adults and an infant were killed in New South Wales allegedly from jack jumper ants or Myrmecia pyri- Brown bears are considered unpredictable.* [33] In 2007, formis.* [6] In 1963 another caused by an ant attack docu- a fatality occurred in Finland from an attack by a Euromented in Tasmania.* [7]* [8] Identification of venom al- pean Brown Bear.* [34] Typically one or two people are lergens began in the early 1990s.* [9] all in Tasmania and attacked rather groups, with no attacks being recorded all due to anaphylactic shock.* [10]* [11]* [12]* [lower- against groups of more than seven.* [35] alpha 1] The fatality rate was one person every four years from the sting.* [14] Grizzly bear Bees A grizzly bear entered the home of a couple and killed a * Africanized honey bees are known to attack people un- woman in January 2015. [36] Two people were attacked but survived in glacier national Park in 2005.* [37] provoked.* [15]* [16]* [17]* [18] Hornets 2.2.4 Beavers Giant Asian Hornets in China have killed at least 42 peoMain article: Beaver attack ple injured 1,675 more.* [19]* [20] 2.2.3 Bears Beaver attacks are uncommon but are becoming more frequent. Beavers aggressively defend their territory.* [38] They may attack humans when suffering from rabies,* [39]* [40] Beavers will attack on land or water. The front incisors are particularly sharp, and have passed through limbs and caused serious blood loss.* [41] One beaver attack was known to be fatal when a 60-yearold fisherman in Europe had his artery bit open in his leg.* [40] the attack was described as “the latest in a series of beaver attacks on humans in the country”, where a growing beaver population has increased its interactions with people.* [40] 162 bear attacks were reported in the United States between 1900 and 1985. This is about two reported bear attacks per year.* [21] During the 1990s bears killed around three people a year in the U.S. and Canada.* [21]* [22] A black bear killed three teenagers in Algonquin Park in Canada.* [23] The majority of attacks happened in national parks.* [24] 1028 incidences of black bears acting aggressively toward people, 107 of which resulted in injury, were recorded from 1964 to 1976 in the Great Smoky Mountains National Park.* [23] After a 20-year Non-fatal beaver attacks have included: an attack on a ban, Florida is considering legislation that may permit man swimming in Dobra River, Croatia.* [42] an attack bear hunting to stop the expanding population of black in saltwater on a snorkeler off the coast of Canada;* [41] bears that are a menace in suburban neighborhoods.* [25] the attack and biting of a woman in Virginia by a rabid beaver;* [39] and an attack on a Boy Scout leader in Pennsylvania.* [43] Asian black bears Asian black bears are comparatively more aggressive toward humans than those of Europe or Asia.* [26] In India, attacks have increased. These occur near the Himalayan region. Here, attacks increased from 10 in 1988–89 to 21 in 1991–92.* [27] Recent bear attacks on humans have been reported from Junbesi National Park and Langtang National Park in Nepal, and occurred in villages as well as in the surrounding forest.* [28] Li Guoxing, the second person in history to have received a facial transplant, was a victim of a black bear attack.* [29]* [30] 2.2.5 Birds Cassowary 1926 – a 16-year-old Queensland boy* [44]* [45] Magpies Between 1979–1989 nine people were killed in Japan Main article: Australian magpie § Swooping .* [31] In 2009 one bear attacked a group of tourists in 2.2. ANIMAL ATTACKS 59 2.2.10 Cougars Ostrich 1997-A woman in South Africa was killed while walking Main article: List of fatal cougar attacks in North America through a field on an ostrich farm.* [46] Roosters 2009 - A five-year-old boy was attacked while hiking with his family.* [72] Roosters have been the cause of some fatalities, usually by a bird during a match.* [47]* [48]* [49]* [50] Roosters 2.2.11 Coyotes have killed babies.* [51]* [52] Main article: Coyote attacks on humans Swan Coyote attacks are uncommon and usually cause little harm but have become more frequent. This is especially Swans are large birds and are able to cause significant true in California. Beginning 30 years prior to 2006 harm.* [53] one hundred sixty took place mostly in the Los Angeles County region.* [73] 41 attacks occurred during 1988– 1997, 48 attacks were verified from 1998 through 2003. • 2012 - Kyaker killed in Chicago.* [54] The majority of these incidents occurred in Southern California.* [74] Some coyotes chase joggers and bicyclists, • 2009 - boaters were attacked by a swan that jumped confront people walking their dogs, and stalk small chilinto their boats and attacked with its beak. One boat dren.* [74] capsized.* [55] 2.2.6 Bulls Cattle have killed people* [56] but bulls are more likely to attack and kill people in a variety of contexts. Interacting with the bull is part of some the sports in some cultures. This sometimes results in the death by bull attack.* [57] Bulls attack and kill people on farms.* [58]* [59]* [60] In 2010, a couple were passing through a field where a bull was pastured and the man was killed.* [61] • 2009 – a Canadian was killed by three coyotes.* [75]* [76] • 2013 - a three-year-old mistook a coyote as a dog and was bitten in the face.* [77] • 2015 - a man working in his garden was attacked.* [78] 2.2.12 Crocodiles Main article: Crocodile attack Crocodile attacks often result in fatalities.* [79] Estimates of deaths due to attacks by the The Nile crocodile is estimated be hundreds and possibly thouIn 2012, villagers living near an African game reserve sands yearly.* [80]* [81] Attacks by Nile crocodiles range * were attacked by chimpanzees. One girl was killed. [62] from 275 to 745 per year. 63% of these are fatal. Only 30 attacks have been recorded per year by saltwater crocodiles, of which 50% are fatal. Fatal attacks are typ2.2.8 Catfish ically made by very large crocodiles are considered to be predatory. The Nile crocodile is considered to be the 1998 through 2007 – Three young Indian most prolific predator of humans among wild animals at men.* [63]* [64]* [65] this time.* [82] Crocodile tracking technology is currently under development that would prevent attacks.* [83] 2.2.7 Chimpanzees 2.2.9 Cats 2.2.13 Dogs Though very rare cats have attacked and killed people.* [66]* [67]* [68]* [69]* [70] A medical examiner de- Main article: Dog bite termined that a cat smothered an infant in 1982 and that previous documented cases were probably substan- The numbers of fatalities from dog attacks have not been tiated.* [71] firmly established. Some estimates are 20 to 30 times 60 CHAPTER 2. ARTICLES each year,* [84]* [85] while others set the figure some- Leopard attacks remain a danger in some areas.* [103] where around 186.* [86] The National Health and Human One leopard in India killed over 200 people.* [103] LeopServices agency in the United States reports that 9.9% of ard attacks usually occur at night. deaths caused by animals were from dogs.* [87] 2.2.21 Lions 2.2.14 Elephants Main article: Lion § Man-eating Wild elephants have attacked, harmed and killed people.* [88]* [89] Jacky Boxberger, an Olympic athlete, Bùi Thị Xuân, a Vietnamese woman general and Allen Campbell, a professional elephant trainer were attacked and killed by elephants. Elephants have attacked people in villages in India.* [90] Lions enter areas occupied by humans.* [104] Lion attacks in Tanzania increased from 1990 to 2005. At least 563 villagers were attacked and many eaten over this period. Researchers argue that conservation policy contributes directly to human deaths. Lions have taken peoDuring the past five years there were 37,512 cases of wild ple from the center of large towns. Estimates stand at elephant attacks with 54 casualties in Pu'er, south China's 550–700 people attacked by lions every year.* [105] Yunnan Province* [91] 2.2.22 Racoon 2.2.15 Fox A musician was attacked and had her ankle gnawed upon In 2004, a fox attacked a woman as she exited her home in Central Park.* [106] A blind 10-year-old rescued her friend from an attack.* [107] A Washington State jogger in Scotland.* [92] was attacked.* [108] 2.2.16 Hippopotamus 2.2.23 Rats The Hippopotamus is considered by some to be the most dangerous animal in the world,* [93] killing up to 300 The National Health and Human Services agency in the United States reported 3 fatalities between people each year.* [94]* [95] 1979 and 1990 from rats.* [109] When rats attack it is directed typically toward small children or in2.2.17 Horses fants.* [110]* [111]* [112]* [113] Between 1996 and 2009 the National Institutes for Safety and Health 14 documented deaths resulted from a kick to 2.2.24 Sharks the chest or abdomen by a horse.* [96] Main article: Shark attack 2.2.18 Hyenas Twelve unprovoked shark attacks in Australia occurred in 2004 in Australia, two were fatal.* [114] Attacks on humans by spotted hyenas are underre* ported.* [97] A pair of hyenas were responsible for killing 1791 – an identified Aboriginal woman. [115] 27 people in Mulanje, Malawi in 1962.* [98] In 1910 spotted hyenas were observed to kill sufferers of African 2.2.25 Snakes sleeping sickness as they slept outside in camps.* [99] 2.2.19 Komodo dragons Further information: List of fatal snake bites in the United States A man was bitten and subsequently lost his big toe to a The National Health and Human Services agency in the Komodo dragon.* [100]* [101] Attacks occur infrequently United States reported 66 fatalities between 1979 and in Indonesia.* [102] 1990 from snakes.* [109] 2.2.20 Leopards Main article: Leopard attack African Rock python An African rock python killed two boys in Campbellton, New Brunswick in 2013.* [116] 2.2. ANIMAL ATTACKS 61 • Wolf of Sarlat Pythons Species of python have attacked people and caused human fatalities. These include: • Wolf of Soissons • Wolves of Ashta • Early 19th century – Two people in Indonesia* [117] • Wolves of Hazaribagh • 1910 or 1927 – a man on a hunting trip from Burma.* [118] • Wolves of Paris • 1932 – a Filipino teenager was consumed by his pet.* [119] • Wolves of Périgord • Wolves of Turku • 1995 – a 29-year-old tapper from southern 2.2.28 Malaysia* [119] See also • 2008 – a 25-year-old woman.* [120] • 2010 Sharm el-Sheikh shark attacks • 2009, a 3-year-old Las Vegas boy was attacked but rescued before being asphyxiated.* [121] • 2013 New Brunswick python attack • 2009, a two-year-old Orlando area girl was killed* [122] • Azaria Chamberlain disappearance • Fatal dog attacks in the United States • Jersey Shore shark attacks of 1916 2.2.26 Tigers Main article: Man-eating tigers • Kali River goonch attacks • Kelly Keen coyote attack • Kenton Joel Carnegie wolf attack Tigers kill more people than any other big cat, and tigers have been responsible for more human deaths through direct attack than any other wild mammal.* [123] One hundred twenty-nine people in the Sundarbans from 1969– 71.* [104]* [123] The Sundarbans are occupied by 600 royal Bengal tigers* [124] who before modern times used to “regularly kill fifty or sixty people a year”attacks continue to increase.* [124]* [125] Well-known and documented tigers • Tigers of Chowgarh (1925–30) • List of fatal alligator attacks in the United States • List of fatal bear attacks in North America • List of fatal cougar attacks in North America • List of fatal snake bites in the United States • List of fatal shark attacks in the United States • List of shark attacks in South African territorial waters • List of wolf attacks in North America • Tiger of Mundachipallam • Tiger of Segur • Tigress of Champawat (killed in 1907) • Tigress of Jowlagiri 2.2.27 Wolves Main article: Wolf attacks on humans • Kirov wolf attacks • Wolf of Ansbach • Wolf of Gysinge 2.2.29 Notes [1] The total amount of deaths from this 20 year period due to the ant could be higher. One account reports of another fatality in Tasmania and one in New South Wales.* [13] 2.2.30 References [1] “Injury Facts Chart”. National Safety Council. Retrieved 2015-04-09. [2] Warrell, D.A. (1993). “Venomous bites and stings in the tropical world”. Med J Aust 159: 773–779. [3] Langley, Ricky L.; Morrow, William E. (1997). “Deaths resulting from animal attacks in the United States”. Wilderness and Environmental Medicine 8: 8–16. 62 CHAPTER 2. ARTICLES [4] “Beware of the Bugs: Fire Ants Can Kill Americans”. ABC News. Retrieved 22 March 2015. [5] deShazo RD, Kemp SF, deShazo MD, Goddard J. (2004) Fire ant attacks on patients in nursing homes: an increasing problem. Am J Med. 116(12):843-6. PMID 15178500 [6] Cleland, J.B. (1931). “Insects in Their Relationship to Injury and Disease in Man in Australia. Series III”. The Medical Journal of Australia 2: 711. [7] Trica, J.C. (24 October 1964). “Insect Allergy in Australia: Results of a Five-Year Survey”. The Medical journal of Australia 2: 659–63. PMID 14213613. [20] . CNN http://edition.cnn.com/2013/10/03/world/asia/ hornet-attack-china/index.html?hpt=hp_c3. Retrieved 2015-04-14. Missing or empty |title= (help) [21] Cardall, Taylor Y. and Peter Rosen. “Grizzly Bear Attack”The Journal of Emergency Medicine p. 331. [22] Fergus, Charles. Wild Guide: Bears, p. 97 [23] Hunter and hunted: relationships between carnivores and people by Hans Kruuk, published by Cambridge University Press, 2002, ISBN 0-521-89109-4 [24] A Book of Man Eaters by Brigadier General R. G. Burton, Mittal Publications [8] Clarke, PS (December 1986). “The natural history of sensitivity to jack jumper ants (Hymenoptera formicidae Myrmecia pilosula) in Tasmania.”. The Medical journal of Australia 145 (11–12): 564–6. PMID 3796365. Retrieved 29 November 2014. [25] “Florida Mulls Return Of Bear Hunts After Increase In Urban Attacks”. Huffington Post. Retrieved 2015-04-14. [9] Ford, SA; Baldo, BA; Weiner, J; Sutherland, S (March 1991). “Identification of jack-jumper ant (Myrmecia pilosula') venom allergens”. Clinical and experimental allergy : journal of the British Society for Allergy and Clinical Immunology 21 (2): 167–71. doi:10.1111/j.13652222.1991.tb00826.x. PMID 2043985. [27]“Human-bear interactions”from Status and management of the Asiatic black bear in India by S. Sathyakumar in Asiatic Black Bear Conservation Action Plan, chapter 10 of Bears: Status Survey and Conservation Action Plan compiled by Christopher Servheen, Stephen Herrero and Bernard Peyton, published by IUCN/SSC Bear Specialist Group [10] Brown, SG; Franks, RW; Baldo, BA; Heddle, RJ (January 2003). “Prevalence, severity, and natural history of jack jumper ant venom allergy in Tasmania”. The Journal of allergy and clinical immunology 111 (1): 187–92. doi:10.1067/mai.2003.48. PMID 12532117. [11] “Jumper Ants (Myrmecia pilosula species group)". Australian Venom Research Unit. University of Melbourne. Retrieved 24 August 2014. [12] “Bull and Jumper Ants”. Queensland Museum. Retrieved 24 August 2014. [13] McGain, Forbes; Winkel, Kenneth D. (August 2002). “Ant sting mortality in Australia”. Toxicon 40 (8): 1095– 1100. doi:10.1016/S0041-0101(02)00097-1. PMID 12165310. [14] Guest, Annie (17 February 2005).“Vaccine underway in Tas' for 'Jack jumper' ant bite”. The World Today (Australian Broadcasting Corporation). Retrieved 5 December 2014. [15] “Neighbors react to killer bee swarm in Desert Hot Springs”. News Channel 3, Palm Springs, CA. Retrieved 2015-04-06. |first1= missing |last1= in Authors list (help) [16] [17] [18] [19] [26] Bear Anatomy and Physiology from Gary Brown's The Great Bear Almanac, Lyons & Burford, Publishers, 1993 [28] Status of Asiatic black bears in protected areas of Nepal and the effects of political turmoil [29] “Xinhua – English”. Archived from the original on 14 October 2007. Retrieved 25 November 2007. [30] "'First face transplant' for China”. BBC News. 14 April 2006. Archived from the original on 14 October 2007. Retrieved 25 November 2007. [31] Knight, John (2000). Natural Enemies: People-Wildlife conflicts in Anthropological Perspective. p. 254. ISBN 0415-22441-1. [32] “BBC NEWS – Asia-Pacific – Bear attacks tourists in Japan”. Retrieved 20 December 2014. [33] Bear Attacks: Their Causes and Avoidance by Stephen Herrero, Hurtig Publishers Ltd./ Edmonton 1985 [34] De Giorgio, Fabio; Rainio, Juha; Lalu, Kaisa (15 November 2007). “There attack-A unique fatality in Finland”. Forensic Science International 173 (1): 64–67. [35] “Alaska Bear Incident Update: NOLS Students Injured in Bear Attack in Alaska”. National Outdoor Leadership “Killer Bee Attack Leaves 1 Dead, 4 Injured In Arizona” School. Retrieved 30 January 2014. . Huffington Post. Retrieved 2015-04-06. [36] “Fatal Yukon grizzly bear attack continues to mystify offi“Killer Bees Kill Texas Couple”. Huffington Post. 21 cials”. Alaska Dispatch News. Retrieved 26 March 2015. April 2011. Retrieved 2015-04-06. [37] “A hike into horror and an act of courage in Glacier Na“Killer Bees Attack Florida Man And Firefighters; Kill tional Park”. LA Times. Retrieved 26 March 2015. Dog”. Huffington Post. Retrieved 2015-04-14. [38] Boonstra, R (2013). “Beaver”. Canadian Encyclopedia. “Giant Asian Hornets Are Killing People In China, Breeding In Larger Numbers”. Huffington Post. Re- [39] Huget, Jennifer LaRue (6 September 2012). “Beavers trieved 2015-04-14. and rabies”. Washington Post. 2.2. ANIMAL ATTACKS 63 [40] “Beaver kills man in Belarus”. The Guardian. Associated Press. 29 May 2013. [61] “Man charged after bull kills rambler”. The Guardian. 19 September 2013. Retrieved 21 March 2015. [41] Beswick, Aaron (4 September 2014). “Angry beaver attacks man on Nova Scotia's eastern shore”. The Chronicle Herald. [62] “Amid armed conflict, Congolese villagers face chimpanzee attacks”. The FRANCE 24 Observers. 26 September 2012. Retrieved 2015-04-06. [42] “Karlovčanina ugrizao dabar dok se kupao u Dobri”. Jutarnji list (in Croatian). 26 July 2013. [63] Lucy Cockcroft (9 October 2008).“Mutant fish develops a taste for human flesh in India”. Telegraph.co.uk. [43] “Scouts stone rabid beaver to death after attack”. CBS. 13 August 2012. [64] Cockcroft, Lucy.“Mutant fish develops a taste for human flesh in India”. The Daily Telegraph. Retrieved 25 March 2015. [44] Christensen, Liana (2011). Deadly Beautiful: Vanishing Killers of the Animal Kingdom. Wollombi, NSW: Exisle Publishing. p. 272. ISBN 978-1-921497-22-3. [45] Kofron, Christopher P., Chapman, Angela. (2006) “Causes of mortality to the endangered Southern Cassowary Casuarius casuariusjohnsonii in Queensland, Australia.”Pacific Conservation Biology vol. 12: 175–179 [46] “Ostrich Kicks A Woman To Death In South Africa”. Chicago Tribune. Retrieved 2015-04-08. [47] “Rooster Kills Man Attending Cockfight”. CBS Los Angeles. Retrieved 21 March 2015. [48] “Cockfighting Gone Wrong, Man Killed By Rooster”. Joe For America. Retrieved 21 March 2015. [49] “Man stabbed to death by cockfighting bird”. BBC News. 8 February 2011. Retrieved 19 March 2011. [65] “Kali River goonch attacks”. Bing Knows. Retrieved 25 March 2015. [66] “Brett Nash cat electrocution case: Can a cat kill you?". Retrieved 21 March 2015. [67] “DEPRIVED OF KITTENS CAT ATTACKS BABY Mother Terribly Clawed - Father and Two Policemen Hurt in Killing It.”. New York Times. Retrieved 27 March 2015. [68] “Woman after house cat attack: 'I was mauled for the kill'". Seattle News. Retrieved 21 March 2015. [69] “Schenectady Gazette – Google News Archive Search”. google.com. [70] “Carbondale Daily Free Press, September 16, 1907, Page 2”. newspaperarchive.com. [50] Peralta, Eyder (7 February 2011). “Weird News: California Man Fatally Stabbed By Rooster : The Two-Way” [71] Kearny, Michael S; Dahl, Lauritz B; Stalsberg, Helge (18 September 1982). “Can a cat smother and kill a baby?". . NPR. Retrieved 26 September 2013. British Medical Journal 285. [51] “Rooster Kills Baby”. Crittenden Record-Press from Marion, Kentucky. Retrieved 21 March 2015. [72] “Boy Attacked, Dragged By Unknown Animal While Hiking”. Huffington Post. Retrieved 2015-04-14. [52] “Rooster Kills”. The Weekly Examiner. Retrieved 21 March 2015. [73] Dell'Amore, Christine (March 2006). “City Slinkers”. Smithsonian. Smithsonian. Retrieved 14 June 2012. [53] “Who, What, Why: How dangerous are swans?". BBC News. Retrieved 25 March 2015. [74] “Coyote Attacks: An Increasing Suburban Problem”. Archived from the original on 26 September 2007. Re[54] “Mute Swan Attacks and Kills Man on Kayak in Chicago trieved 19 August 2007. Pond”. Field & Stream. Retrieved 21 March 2015. [55] Gammell, Caroline. "'Mr Asbo' the swan attacks unsuspecting rowers”. The Daily Telegraph. Retrieved 201504-09. [75] “Coyotes kill Toronto singer in Cape Breton”. CBC.ca. 12 October 2009. Retrieved 29 October 2009. [76] A History of Urban Coyote Problems, Robert M. Tim & Rex O. Baker, University of Nebraska–Lincoln, 2007 [56] “Fatalities Caused by Cattle --- Four States, 2003̶2008” . CDC. Retrieved 22 March 2015. [77] “Emeil Hawkins, 3-Year-Old Chicago Boy, Attacked By Coyote After Mistaking It For Dog”. Huffington Post. [57]“LiveLeak.com – Bull kills man in Colombia”. LiveLeak. Retrieved 2015-04-14. Retrieved 21 March 2015. [58] “Union County man killed by bull”. The Columbus Dispatch. Retrieved 21 March 2015. [78] NORMAN, JIM.“Coyote euthanized after it attacks Saddle River man”. NorthJersey.com. Retrieved 2015-0408. [59] Duncan, Dustin.“Bull kills man in Paducah”. The Southern News. Retrieved 21 March 2015. [79] Egerton, p. 298. [60] “Bull kills man in Iowa County”. WXOW News 19 La Crosse, WI. Retrieved 21 March 2015. [80] Wood, The Guinness Book of Animal Facts and Feats. Sterling Pub Co Inc (1983), ISBN 978-0-85112-235-9 64 CHAPTER 2. ARTICLES [81] Caldicott, David G.E.; Croser, David; Manolis, Charlie; [98] Webb, Grahame; Britton, Adam (1 September 2005). “Crocodile Attack in Australia: An Analysis of Its Incidence and Review of the Pathology and Management of Crocodilian Attacks in General”. Wilderness & Envi- [99] ronmental Medicine 16 (3): 143–159. doi:10.1580/10806032(2005)16[143:CAIAAA]2.0.CO;2. PMID 16209470. Retrieved 3 February 2013. [100] [82]“Crocodilian Attacks” . IUCN Crocodile Specialist Group (iucncsg.org). Retrieved 3 February 2013. [83] “Tracking crocodile movements could reduce attacks on humans, researchers say”. The Sydney Morning Herald. [84] Langley, Ricky L. (March 2009). “Human Fatalities Resulting From Dog Attacks in the United States, 1979– 2005”. Wilderness & Environmental Medicine 20 (1): 19–25. doi:10.1580/08-WEME-OR-213.1. Kruuk, Hans (2002) Hunter and hunted: relationships between carnivores and people Cambridge University Press, ISBN 0-521-89109-4 Roosevelt, Theodore (1910) African Game Trails: An Account of the African Wanderings of an American Hunter, Naturalist, New York, C. Scribner's sons Cagle, Jess (23 June 2001). “Transcript: Sharon Stone vs. the Komodo Dragon”. TIME. Retrieved 12 February 2010. [101] “Lizard bites Sharon Stone's husband”. BBC News. 11 June 2001. Retrieved 12 February 2010. [102] Markus Makur, 'Giant lizard attack: Komodo dragon bites elderly woman on Rinca island', The Jakarta Post, 13 October 2012. 2 hospitalized in Indonesia after Komodo attack', The Jakarta Post, 7 February 2013. [103] Nowak, Ronald M; and Paradiso, John L. Walker's Mammals of the World. 4th ed. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins [85] Sacks, Jeffrey J., MD, MPH; Sinclair, Leslie, DVM; University Press; 1983. p1090 Gilchrist, Julie, MD; Golab, Gail C., PhD, DVM; Lockwood, Randall, PhD. (15 September 2000). “Breeds of [104] John Seidensticker and Susan Lumpkin (1991). Great dogs involved in fatal human attacks in the United States Cats. p. 240. ISBN 0-87857-965-6. between 1979 and 1998”. JAVMA 217 (6): 836–40. doi:10.2460/javma.2000.217.836. PMID 10997153. [105] Packer, C.; Ikanda, D.; Kissui, B.; Kushnir, H. (August 2005). “Conservation biology: lion attacks on [86] Langley humans in Tanzania”. Nature 436 (7053): 927–928. doi:10.1038/436927a. PMID 16107828. [87] Langley, p.10 [106] “Central Park Raccoon Attack Lands Taraka Larson, [88] “British teacher gored by elephant on South African saBrooklyn Woman, In Hospital”. Huffington Post. Refari”. The Daily Telegraph. Retrieved 25 March 2015. trieved 2015-04-14. [89] “Elephants Attack Indian Village”. Daily Mail Online. [107] “Piper Rolfe, Legally Blind Vermont Girl, Saves Friend Retrieved 25 March 2015. . From Vicious Raccoon Attack”. Huffington Post. Retrieved 2015-04-14. [90] “Deadly herd of elephants attacks village in India”. Fox [108] “Michaela Lee, Washington Woman, Attacked By RacNews Channel. Retrieved 25 March 2015. coons”. Huffington Post. Retrieved 2015-04-14. [91] “At Least 6 Fatal Wild Elephant Attacks in China Last [109] Langley, p.9 Year”. CRIENGLISH.com. Retrieved 25 March 2015. [92] “Pensioner attacked by fox in her back garden”. The Scotsman. Retrieved 2015-04-09. [110] “Rat Raised to Feed Pennsylvania Family's Snakes Bites, Kills Baby”. NBC 10 Philadelphia. Retrieved 21 March 2015. [93] Ismael, Zunaid. “Security guard survives hippo attack” [111] “Giant rats eat two babies in South Africa townships in . iafrica.com. Retrieved 25 March 2015. separate attacks”. Daily Mail Online. Retrieved 21 March 2015. [94] “The Straight Dope: Are hippos the most dangerous an[112] “Baby with 100 Small Rat bites Bled to Death”. Fox imal?". The Straight Dope. Retrieved 22 March 2015. News Channel. Retrieved 21 March 2015. [95]“The World's Most Dangerous Animals: Top 10 Deadliest Animals”. Animal Danger. Retrieved 22 March 2015. [113] “Rats, muti blamed for mutilated baby's death”. News24. Retrieved 21 March 2015. [96] “An Overview of Safety and Health for Workers in the [114] International Shark Attack File. 2005. SAF Statistics for Horse-Racing Industry, DHHS (NIOSH) Publication No. the Worldwide Locations with the Highest Shark Attack 2009–128 April 2009”. CDC. p. 6. Retrieved 22 March Activity Since 1990, Florida Museum of Natural History 2015. [115] “aboriginal woman”. A Complete Account of the Settle[97] Begg, Colleen, Begg, Kieth & Muemedi, Oscar (2007) ment at Port Jackson by Watkin Tench. sharkattackfile.net. Preliminary data on human – carnivore conflict in Niassa Retrieved 25 March 2015. National Reserve, Mozambique, particularly fatalities due to lion, spotted hyaena and crocodile, SGDRN (Sociedade [116] Than, K., "Strangulation of Sleeping Boys Puts Spotlight para a Gestão e Desenvolvimento da Reserva do Niassa on Pythons", National Geographic Daily News, 6 August Moçambique) 2013. 2.2. ANIMAL ATTACKS [117] Kopstein, F. (1927): Over het verslinden van menschen door Python reticulatus ["On the swallowing of humans by P. reticulatus"]. Tropische Natuur 4: 65–67. [Article in Dutch] [118] Bruno, Silvio (1998): I serpenti giganti ["The giant snakes"]. Criptozoologia 4: 16–29. (in Italian) Accessed 22 March 2015 [119] Kobis I. 1995. Giant python killed after trying to swallow man. The Star (Malaysian English newspaper), Axcessed 22 March 2015. [120] Woman killed by pet 13-foot python at UPI. Accessed 22 March 2015. [121] In Las Vegas, python vs. angry mom with a knife at Las Vegas Sun. Accessed 22 March 2015. [122] “12- foot python kills toddler”. Examiner. Retrieved 2015-04-08. [123] Nowak, Ronald M; and Paradiso, John L. Walker's Mammals of the World. 4th ed. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press; 1983. p1088 [124] Loadstar. “Man-eaters. The tiger and lion, attacks on humans.”. lairweb.org.nz. [125] “Tiger attacks on rise in Indian Sundarbans”. DNA India. Indo-Asian News Service. 30 July 2008. Retrieved 11 March 2014. 2.2.31 65 •“Death Statistics Comparison”. UnitedJustice.com. 7 December 2008. 7 December 2008. • Driscoll, Jamus.“Bears on the Rampage”. Outdoor Life 197.2 (1996): 20. • The Fear of Wolves – review of wolfs (sic) attacks on humans • Fergus, Charles. Wild Guide: Bears. Mechanisburg, PA; Stackpole Books, 2005. • Guo, Shuzhong, et al. “Human facial allotransplantation: a 2-year follow-up study”. The Lancet 372.9639 (2008): 631–638. • Masterson, Linda. Living with Bears. Masonville, CO; PixyJack Press, LLC, 2006. • Simmons, Shraga. “Olympic Champions”. aish.com 22 August 2004. 17 November 2008. •“Teeth”. The Internet Encyclopedia of Science: Anatomy & Physiology. 17 November 2008. • Ward, Paul and Suzanne Kynaston. Wild Bears of the World. United Kingdom: Cassell plc, 1995 • Whitman, David. “The Return of the Grizzly”. Atlantic Monthly 286.3 (2000): 26–31. Bibliography • Egerton, L. ed. 2005. Encyclopedia of Australian 2.2.32 External links wildlife. Reader's Digest ISBN 1-876689-34-X • “Fatal Mountain Lion Attacks”. Southeastern Out•“The Man-Eater of Jowlagiri”, from Nine Mandoors. Retrieved 31 August 2007. Eaters and One Rogue, Kenneth Anderson, Allen & Unwin, 1955 • “List of Mountain Lion Attacks on People in Cal• CrocBITE • “Fatal Alligator Attacks”. Southeastern Outdoors. Retrieved 31 March 2006. • “Alligator Attacks Fact sheet, p.4-5 (updated 11/29/05)" (PDF). Florida Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission. Archived from the original on 8 May 2006. Retrieved 12 April 2006. • Anitei, Stefan. “The Limits of the Human Nose: How much can a human smell?" Softpedia. 22 January 2007. 17 November 2008 • Batin, Christopher. “Bear Attacks!" Outdoor Life 210.6 (2003): 46. • Brandt, Anthony. “Attack”. Outdoor Life 197.1 (1996): 52. • Cardall, Taylor Y. and Peter Rosen. “Grizzly Bear Attack”. The Journal of Emergency Medicine 24.3 (2003): 331–333. ifornia”. Retrieved 31 August 2007. • “List of Confirmed Cougar Attacks In the United States and Canada 1890 – 1990”. Retrieved 2 September 2007. • “List of Confirmed Cougar Attacks In the United States and Canada 1991 – 2000”. Retrieved 1 September 2007. • NCIPC bibliography of articles on dog bites • Dogs Bite but Balloons and Slippers are More Dangerous by Janis Bradley, 2005 • CDC Dog Bite Factsheet • “List of Confirmed Cougar Attacks in the United States and Canada 2001 – 2010”. Retrieved 5 May 2012. • Global Shark Attack File 66 CHAPTER 2. ARTICLES 2.3 Dog bite 2.3.2 Causes Human behavior “Dog Bite”redirects here. For the song, see Dog Bite (song). Many human behaviors (especially by people unfamiliar with dogs) may factor into bite situations. The majority of Dog bites or dog attacks are attacks on humans by feral dogs will not respond to all or even any of these behaviors or domestic dogs. With the close association of dogs and with aggression, however, some will. These behaviors inhumans in daily life (largely as pets), dog bites – with clude: injuries from very minor to significant – are extremely common. • Challenging for food or water. For example, removing food from a dog, or appearing to intervene beThere is considerable debate on whether or not certain tween a dog and its food. Even when inadvertent, breeds of dogs are inherently more prone to commit atthis may trigger aggressive behavior in some anitacks causing serious injury (i.e., so driven by instinct and mals. breeding that, under certain circumstances, they are exceedingly likely to attempt or commit dangerous attacks). Regardless of the breed of the dog, it is recognized that the risk of dangerous dog attacks can be greatly increased by human actions (such as neglect or fight training) or inactions (as carelessness in confinement and control). Significant dog bites affect tens of million of people globally each year.* [1] It is estimated that two percent of the US population, from 4.5–4.7 million people, are bitten by dogs each year.* [2] Most bites occur in children.* [3] In the 1980s and 1990s the US averaged 17 fatalities per year, while in the 2000s this has increased to 26.* [4] 77% of dog bites are from the pet of family or friends, and 50% of attacks occur on the dog owner's property.* [4] Animal bites, most of which are from dogs, are the reason for 1% of visits to an emergency department in the United States.* [3] Fifty percent of the payout of home insurance due to attacks committed by homeowners' dogs. Attacks on the serious end of the spectrum have become the focus of increasing media and public attention in the late 20th and early 21st centuries.* [5] 2.3.1 Health effects • Attacking (or perceived attacking) a dog or its companions, or encroaching on its territory. Dogs are pack hunters; they often have an instinct to defend themselves and those they consider their “pack” (which could be other dogs, humans, cats, or even other animals), and to defend their territory, which may include areas they consider“theirs”or belonging to their family. Any dog is unpredictable in the presence of an intruder, especially but not always a burglar. • Sickness or injury. A sick or injured dog, or an older animal, like people, may become “cranky” or over-reactive, and may develop a tendency to become “snappish”. • Failure to recognize insecurity or fear. Like humans, dogs that feel insecure may ultimately turn and defend themselves against perceived threat. It is common for people to not recognize signs of fear or insecurity, and to approach, triggering a defensive reaction. • Intervention when dogs fight. When dogs fight, a human stepping in between, or seeking to restrain one of them without due care, may be badly bitten as well. Rabies results in the death of approximately 55,000 people a year, with most of the causes due to dog bites.* [1] • Threatening body language. Especially including direct staring (an act of aggression/perceived as threatening by dogs) or a person not known to the dog moving their face very close to the animal's own snout (may be perceived as a challenge, threatening, or imposing). Staring is more dangerous when on the same visual level as the dog (such as small children), or when the human is unfamiliar. Capnocytophaga canimorsus transmission (a gramnegative bacterium) following a dog bite can cause overwhelming sepsis in asplenic patients, the elderly, and the immunocompromised. Empiric treatment for this bacteria following a dog bite, consisting of a thirdgeneration cephalosporins early in the infection, should be instituted in these patient populations, or following deep bites or dog bites to the hand. • Prey behaviors. Dogs retain many of the predatory instincts of wolves, including the chasing of prey. Running away from a dog or behaving in a manner suggesting weakness may trigger predatory behaviors such as chasing or excited attack. For example, the instinct to jerk one's hands upwards away from an inquisitive dog may elicit a strong impulse to grab and hold. A person bitten by an animal potentially carrying parvovirus or rabies virus should consult a medical care. An animal bite may also cause serious bacterial infections of soft tissues or bone (osteomyelitis) which can become life-threatening if untreated. 2.3. DOG BITE 67 • Ignoring warning signs. Trained attack dogs may act Training and aggression against an intruder without warning. In a domestic situation, canine aggression is normally Note that attacks may be triggered by behaviors that are suppressed. Exceptions are if the dog is trained to atperceived as an attack, for example, a sudden unexpected tack, feels threatened, or is provoked. It is important approach or touch by a stranger, or inadvertently stepping to remember that dogs are predators by nature, instinct on any portion of the dog's anatomy, such as a paw or tail, is something that never completely disappears, and that or startling a sleeping dog unexpectedly. In particular, the predatory behavior against other animals (such as chasterritory that a dog recognizes as its own may not coin- ing other animals) may train a dog or a pack of dogs to cide with the property lines that its owner and the legal attack humans. It is possible to acclimate a dog to comauthorities recognize, such as a portion of a neighbor's mon human situations in order to avoid adverse reactions backyard. by a pet Dog experts advocate removal of a dog's food, startling a dog, and performing sudden movements in a controlled setting to teach the dog who its leader is, to Dog behavior defuse aggressive impulses in common situations. This Many adoption agencies test for aggressive behavior in also allows better animal care since owners may now redogs, and euthanize an animal that shows certain types move an article directly from a dog's mouth or transport of aggression. Alternatively, aggression can often be ad- a wounded pet to seek medical attention. dressed with appropriate corrective training. Sources of Small children are especially prone to being misunderaggression include: stood by dogs, in part because their size and movements can be similar to prey.* [8] Also, young children may un• Fear and self-defense. Like humans, dogs react intentionally provoke a dog (pulling on ears or tails is when fearful, and may feel driven to attack out of common, as is surprising a sleeping dog) because of their self-defense, even when not in fact being“attacked” inexperience. To avoid potential conflicts, even reliably . Speed of movement, noises, objects or specific well-behaved children and dogs should never be allowed gestures such as raising an arm or standing up may to interact in the absence of an adult who knows and unelicit a reaction. Many rescued dogs have been derstands the dog's personality and trained cues. abused, and in some dogs, specific fears of men, Dogs with strong chase instincts, (e.g. collies, shepherds), women, skin coloring, and other features that recall may fail to recognize a person as a being not to be herded. past abusers, are not uncommon. A dog that feels They may fixate on a specific aspect of the person, such cornered or without recourse may attack the human as a fast-moving, brightly colored shoe, as a prey obwho is threatening or attacking it. A dog may also ject. This is probably the cause for the majority of nonperceive a hand reached out toward its head as an aggressive dogs chasing cyclists and runners. In these attempt to gain control of the dog's neck via the colcases, if the individual stops, the dog often loses interlar, which if done to a wary dog by a stranger can est since the movement has stopped. This is not always easily provoke a bite. the case, and aggressive or territorial dogs might take the • Territoriality and possessions. See above. Aggres- opportunity to attack. sive possessiveness is considered a very important Additionally, most dogs that bark at strangers, particutype of aggression to test for, since it is most associ- larly when not on“their”territory, will flee if the stranger ated with bites, especially bites to children.* [6] challenges it, though this is not recommended behaviour • Predatory instincts. In isolation, predatory behaviors are rarely the cause of an attack on a human. Predatory aggression is more commonly involved as a contributing factor for example in attacks by multiple dogs; a“pack kill instinct”may arise if multiple dogs are involved in an attack.* [7] as challenging the dog is just as likely to evoke a bite. Mailmen, being the classic example, provoke a strong territorial response because they come back day after day to the dog's territory. In the dog's mind they are constantly intruding on their territory and that sets up a learned behavior. • Pain or sickness. See above. As with fear, pain can incite a dog to attack. The canonical example Unsupervised children of sickness-induced attack is the virulent behavior caused by rabies. This is arguably the most critical factor in fatal dog attacks • Redirected aggression. A dog that is already ex- on children, who because of their small size are usually cited/aroused by an aggressive instinct from one not able to withstand an attack until help arrives. Many source, may use an available target to release its ag- adults survived severe dog attacks simply by virtue of the gression, if the “target”does something to evoke fact that they were able to sustain and fend the dogs off to this response from the dog (e.g. shouting & staring some degree until assistance arrived, although the elderly at the dog for barking at the mailman). and disabled are particularly vulnerable. 68 CHAPTER 2. ARTICLES Children often engage in behavior that will trigger a dog attack. For example, approaching a chained dog, trying to hug or kiss an unfamiliar animal, trying to pull its tail or engaging in other behavior that the dog may feel is threatening. Behavior such as this on the part of children may invoke either an aggressive territorial response from the dog or an aggressive defensive behavior from the dog. The age group with the second-highest amount of fatalities due to a dog attack are 2-year-old children. Over 88% of these fatalities occurred when the 2-year-old child was left unsupervised with a dog(s) or the child wandered off to the location of the dog. 2.3.3 Breed-specific attacks cases of fatal dog attacks; of which "pit bull terrier" or mixes thereof were reportedly involved in 76 cases. The breed with the next-highest number of attributed fatalities was the Rottweiler and mixes thereof, with 44 fatalities.* [2] A study based on more recent data (2000-2009), published in 2013, compared media accounts with reports available from animal control officials, determined that, of their sample of 256 dog bite related fatalities, breed could only be validly determined in 45 cases, and the attacks in these 45 cases were dispersed among 20 different breeds and 2 known mixes. For a further set of 401 dogs in media accounts of dog bite related fatality, reported breed differed between different media accounts of the same attack 31% of the time, factoring in animal control accounts produced disagreement on breed for 40% of attacks.* [11] Dog attacks on humans that appear most often in the news are those that require the hospitalization of the victim or those in which the victim is killed. Dogs of all sizes have 2.3.4 Human-dog interaction mauled and killed humans, although large dogs are capaSee also: Anthrozoology ble of inflicting more damage quickly. When dogs are near humans with whom they are familiar, they normally become less aggressive. However, it should not be assumed that because a dog has been with humans, it will not attack anybody - even a family member. Caution needs to be taken when approaching new dogs for the first time.* [9] Despite domestication, dogs, like their ancestors wolves, remain cunning, swift, agile, strong, territorial and voracious̶even small ones have large, sharp teeth and claws and powerful muscles in their jaws and legs and can inflict serious injuries. The lacerations even from inadvertent dog scratches, let alone deliberate or reckless bites, Due to the pit bull-type breeds' perceived aggression, are easily infected (most commonly by Capnocytophaga owning such an animal is not allowed in Australia and ochracea or Pasteurella multocida). Medium-to-large many European countries, and in several US and Cana- dogs can knock people down with the usual effects of falls dian localities (see breed-specific legislation for details). from other causes. It is sometimes argued that certain breeds are inherently Should affection or mutual respect not exist (as with feral aggressive towards humans and shouldn't be allowed at dogs), should a dog be deliberately starved (dogs are usuall, or that, due to the popularity of certain potentially ally as resourceful as any large predators in getting food), dangerous breeds, these dogs are often owned by irre- should a dog be conditioned to become an attacker, or sponsible owners who provide insufficient training or, should someone intrude upon a dog's territory and pose a worse, aggressiveness training. An opposing argument is threat, then the natural tendencies of a predator manifest that no breed is inherently aggressive towards humans and themselves in a dog attack in which the dog uses its predathat regulating one breed simply moves the irresponsible tory abilities to defend itself. Extrication from such an owners to start focusing on breeds that haven't yet been attack is difficult because of the dog's power and agility. regulated, moving the problem to other breeds. This is Flight from a dog attack by running is usually impossible. one of the positions taken by the American Veterinary Education for adults and children, animal training, selecMedical Association.* [10] tive breeding for temperament, and society's intolerance Although research and analysis* suggests that breed- for dangerous animals combine to reduce the incidence of specific legislation is not completely effective in prevent- attacks and accidents involving humans and dogs. Howing dog attacks, with each new attack, pressure mounts to ever, improperly managed confrontations can lead to seenact such legislation. vere injury from even the most well-tempered dog. Breed disparity An early study (published 2000) in the US by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) covered the years 1979-1998. The study found reports of 327 people killed by dogs over the 20-year period. The CDC used newspaper articles without corroboration by animal control reports for breed“identifications”in 238 of the 327 Stiffened front legs and a raised ridge of hair along the spine can be signs of an imminent attack (as well as of interest, or anxiety and the start of the dog's “fight or flight”mechanism). A wagging tail often is an attempt to communicate excitement, though a tail held high over the back can signal the dog becoming aroused - either for what humans see as for positive or negative reasons. Dogs also have far superior hearing and olfactory senses than humans, as well as having the advantage of reading 2.3. DOG BITE the body language of other humans and animals. 2.3.5 Legal issues United States 69 An unprovoked attack on a jogger by two Cane Corsos was reported in Lapeer, Michigan. The jogger died. The two owners of the dogs were charged with Second degree murder, which carries a potential sentence of Life in Prison.“The dogs involved in the attack have a history of escaping from their kennel and have bitten at least twice before.”* [18] Although using a firearm against an attacking dog may seem acceptable, laws in the United States which pro2.3.6 See also hibit discharging a firearm in a city, and reckless endangerment may limit the extent to which a person is legally • Coyote attacks on humans able to defend themselves in this way. Taking such ac• Dangerous Dogs Act 1991 tions where the dog/dogs involved were not acting aggressively towards humans may result in legal charges against • Fatal dog attacks in the United States the person who shot the animal. No person in the United States has ever been convicted of a crime for firing a gun • Wolf attacks on humans or using any other weapon to stop or kill a dog that was currently attacking him/her.* [12] About whether an attacking dog could itself be criminally 2.3.7 References liable, the California Court of Appeal for the Third Dis- [1] “Animal bites Fact sheet N°373”. World Health Orgatrict explained: nization. February 2013. Retrieved 5 May 2014. Some state laws hold dog owners liable for the harm or damage that their animal causes to people or other dogs. For example, in recent years, Florida dog bite laws have been changed so that prior vicious tendencies may no longer be needed to prove owner liability.* In Texas, dog attack victims are given two possible ways to prove owner negligence when bringing a personal injury or wrongful death claim. The first option is that of strict liability, whereby a victim and their attorney must prove that either the dog has attacked someone else previously (known as the “one bite law”) or else that the owner should otherwise have known their dog was vicious and/or dangerous. The second option is that of owner negligence, which could be argued in cases as various as dogs being allowed to roam freely around neighborhoods, or parents allowing their children to play with pet dogs while unsupervised. Also in Texas, as of September 1, 2007, `Lillian's Law' has taken effect, whereby the owner of a dog that causes death or serious bodily injury may be charged with a second or third degree felony when the attack takes place outside the dog's normal place of confinement (Texas Health & Safety Code Chapter 882). [2] “Dog Bite Prevention”. CDC. Retrieved 22 April 2013. [3] Ellis, R; Ellis, C (Aug 15, 2014). “Dog and cat bites.” . American family physician 90 (4): 239–43. PMID 25250997. [4] Statistics about dog bites in the USA and elsewhere [5] Reuters (2004-10-13). “Stray dog pack attacks Albanian town”. IOL. Retrieved 2008-01-21. An Albanian town had to call in police and hunters after a pack of 200 stray mountain dogs attacked at least nine people. Headed by a clearly identifiable leader, the snarling pack overran the main street of the small northern town of Mamurras, its mayor said on Wednesday. “Even in the movies I have never seen a horde of 200 stray dogs from the mountains attacking people in the middle of a town,”Anton Frroku said on Wednesday. He said the dogs bit at least nine people, aged from 20 to 60, dragging them to the ground and inflicting serious wounds. [6] John, Bisnar (12-11-2012). “Dog Agressive Posessiveness”. p. 1. Check date values in: |date= (help) [7] Shepard Haven Tips for Stopping a Dog Fight In California, owners are subject to massive civil liabil- [8] “Dog Attack Statistics”. Graves Mclain. ity for attacks by their dogs. The state allows a victim to sue on two strict liability causes of action arising out [9] "Dog-Bite-Related Fatalities -- United States, 1995-1996". Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 1997-05-30. of a single attack̶one created by statute and one arisRetrieved 2008-10-29. * ing from common law. [14] In 1989, the California State Legislature enacted a special administrative hearing pro- [10] Spotlight on Dog Bite Prevention Week cedure just for regulating “menacing dogs,”based on the finding that “dangerous and vicious dogs have be- [11] Patronek, Gary J.; Sacks, Jeffrey J.; Delise, Karen M.; Cleary, Donald V.; Marder, Amy R. (15 December come a serious and widespread threat to the safety and 2013). “Co-occurrence of potentially preventable fac* welfare of citizens of this state.” [15] To help impletors in 256 dog bite–related fatalities in the United States ment it, the Judicial Council of California promulgated a (2000–2009)". Journal of the American Veterinary Medpackage of four forms in 1990.* [16] The notice of hearical Association (Schaumburg, Illinois, USA: American ing bears the warning: “DO NOT BRING THE DOG Veterinary Medical Association) 243 (12): 1726–1736. TO THE HEARING.”* [17] doi:10.2460/javma.243.12.1726. PMID 24299544. 70 CHAPTER 2. ARTICLES [12] Woman's neighbor shoots dog Grass Valley man may face animal cruelty charges 2.4.1 Barking in dogs Dog barking is distinct from wolf barking. Wolf barks represent only 2.3% of all wolf vocalizations* [1] and Priebe v. Nelson, 39 Cal. 4th 1112 (2006). are described as “rare”occurrences.* [2] According to Schassburger, wolves bark only in warning, defense, and See California Food and Agricultural Code Section protest. In contrast, dogs bark in a wide variety of social 31601(a). situations, with acoustic communication in dogs being See California Court Forms MC-600, MC-601, MC-602, described as hypertrophic.* [3] Additionally, while wolf and MC-603 barks tend to be brief and isolated, adult dogs bark in long, rhythmic stanzas. Dogs have been known to bark California Court Form MC-601 for hours on end.* [4] [13] People v. Frazier, 173 Cal. App. 4th 613 (2009). [14] [15] [16] [17] [18] “Couple Whose Dogs Fatally Mauled Jogger Charged With Murder”. WWJ. August 1, 2014. Retrieved August 5, 2014. • ^ Sacks, Jeffrey J., MD, MPH; Sinclair, Leslie, DVM; Gilchrist, Julie, MD; Golab, Gail C., PhD, DVM; Lockwood, Randall, PhD. (September 15, 2000). “Breeds of dogs involved in fatal human attacks in the United States between 1979 and 1998”. JAVMA 217 (6): 836– 40. doi:10.2460/javma.2000.217.836. PMID 10997153. • ^ World Almanac and Book of Facts 1985. Doubleday. • ^ World Almanac and Book of Facts 1988. World Almanac Books. While a distinct reason for the difference is unknown, a strong hypothesis is that the vocal communication of dogs developed due to their domestication.* [4] As evidenced by the farm-fox experiment, the process of domestication alters a breed in more ways than just tameness.* [5] Domesticated breeds show vast physical differences from their wild counterparts, notably an evolution that suggests neoteny, or the retention of juvenile characteristics in adults.* [6] Adult dogs have, for example large heads, floppy ears, and shortened snouts – all characteristics seen in wolf puppies.* [7] The behavior, too, of adult dogs shows puppy-like characteristics: dogs are submissive, they whine, and they frequently bark. The experiment illustrates how selecting for one trait (in this case, tameness) can create profound by-products, both physical and behavioral. The frequency of barking in dogs in relation to wolves could also be the product of the very different social environment of dogs. Dogs live in extraordinarily close range with humans, in many societies kept solely as companion animals. From a very young age, humans tend to be one • ^ “Nonfatal Dog Bite--Related Injuries Treated in of a dogʼs primary social contacts. This captive environHospital Emergency Departments”, CDC MMWR, ment presents very different stimuli than would be found by wolves in the wild. While wolves have vast territories, July 4, 2003. dogs do not. The boundaries of a captive dogʼs territory will be visited frequently by intruders, thus triggering the bark response as a warning. Additionally, dogs densely 2.3.8 External links populate urban areas, allowing more opportunity to meet new dogs and be social. For example, it is possible that • NCIPC bibliography of articles on dog bites kenneled dogs may have increased barking due to a de• Dogs Bite but Balloons and Slippers are More Dan- sire to facilitate social behavior. Dogsʼclose relationship gerous by Janis Bradley, 2005 with humans also renders dogs reliant on humans, even for basic needs. Barking is a way to attract attention, and • CDC Dog Bite Factsheet the behavior is continued by the positive response exhibited by the owners (e.g., if a dog barks to get food and the owner feeds it, the dog is being conditioned to continue 2.4 Bark (sound) said behavior.)* [8] • ^ Breed-Specific Legislation in the United States. Linda S. Weiss, Michigan State University - Detroit College of Law (2001). Animal Legal and Historical Web Center A bark is a sound most commonly produced by dogs. Other animals that make this noise include wolves, pinnipeds, foxes and quolls. Woof is the most common representation in the English language for this sound, especially for large dogs. Other transliterations include the onomatopoeic ruff, arf, au au, bow-wow, and, for small dogs, yip. “Bark”is also a verb that describes the sharp explosive cry of certain animals. 2.4.2 Types of barks Barking in domestic dogs is a controversial topic. While suggested that barking is “non-communicative,”* [9] data exists to show that it may well be a means of expression that became increasingly sophisticated during domestication. However, due to the lack of consensus 2.4. BARK (SOUND) 71 over whether or not dogs actually communicate using their barks, there has not been much work done on categorizing the different types of barking in dogs. That which has been done has been criticized by FeddersenPetersen as “lack[ing] objectivity.”Using sonographic methods, Feddersen-Petersen identified several distinct types of barks, and then analyzed them for meanings, functions, and emotions. He separated dog barks into subgroups based on said sonographic data: stery, barked. “One owner thought the scent was preferable to her dog's body odor.”* [13] There is some evidence that humans can determine the suspected emotions of dogs while listening to barks emitted during specific situations. Humans scored the emotions of dogs performing these barks very similarly and in ways that made sense according to the situation at hand. In one example, when subjects were played a recording of a dog tied alone to a tree, a situation in which one could reasonably infer that the dog would be distressed, the human listeners tended to rank the bark as having a high level of despair. It has been suggested that this may be evidence for the idea that dog barks have evolved to be a form of communication with humans specifically, since humans can so readily determine a dog's needs by simply listening to their vocalizations.* [11] Further studies have found that the acoustic structure of a bark "[varies] considerably with context.”* [12] These studies suggest that barks are more than just random sounds, and indeed hold some sort of communicative purpose. 2. Using positive training methods to correct the behaviour. Dogs may bark from anxiety or stress, so punishment can often cause problems by reinforcing a cycle of bad behaviour. Positive approaches can include: Dog barking can be a nuisance to neighbours, and is a common problem that dog owners or their neighbours may face. Many dogs can bark at 100 dBA. Even at 17.5 yards away and with the dog outside a closed window, the noise level of a barking dog can be well over the level that causes psychological distress.* [14] Different kinds of barking often require different kinds of approach to reNot all breeds demonstrated every subgroup of barking. duction. Instead, significant variance in vocalization was found be- Common approaches are as follows: tween different breeds. Poodles showed the least of all barking subunits. Additionally, barking in wolves was ob1. Attempting to understand, and if possible eliminate, served as notably less diverse. For example, wolf barks * the causes of barking. are rarely harmonic, tending instead to be noisy. [10] 2.4.3 Barking as nuisance Bark control Nuisance-barking dogs sound off for no particular reason. “Many dogs bark when they hear other dogs barking,”says Katherine A. Houpt, V.M.D., Ph.D., director of the Cornell Animal Behavior Clinic. Nuisance, inappropriate, or excessive barking comprises between 13 and 35 percent of behavior-problem complaints by dog owners, Houpt noted. The electric collars deliver an irritating shock of adjustable intensity when a vibration sensor in the collar detects barking. The citronella collar releases a spray of citronella when a microphone in the collar senses barking. For the eight dogs that wore both types of collars (one shepherd mix did not complete the study), all owners found the citronella collar to be effective in reducing or stopping nuisance barking and most preferred the fragrance spray. Four out of eight owners said electric shocks had no effect on their dogs̶they kept on barking. The citronella collars had problems, Juarbe-D'az noted. One dog owner complained that citronella oil stained the upholstery when the dog, fond of lying about on uphol- • Repeated exposure to stimuli whilst calming the dog and persuading it to remain quiet. • Distraction as the stimulus happens, through treats, praise, or similar. • Reshaping via clicker training (a form of operant conditioning) or other means to obtain barking behaviour on command, and then shaping the control to gain command over silence. 3. Seeking professional advice from local organizations, dog trainers, or veterinarians. 4. Use of a mechanical device such as a bark collar. There are several types, all of which use a collar device that produces a response to barking that the dog notices: • Citrus spray (“citronella”) - dogs as a rule do not like citrus. At the least, it is very noticeable and disrupts the pattern through surprise. These collars spray citrus around the dog's muzzle when it barks. (Sometimes these devices make a“hissing”noise before spraying, as an additional deterrent - see “Combination and escalation devices”below) • Sonic/ultrasonic (including vibration) - these collars produce a tone which humans may or may not be able to hear, in response to barking. Over time, the sound becomes annoying or distracting enough to deter barking. • Electrical - these collars produce a mild stinging or tingling sensation in response to a bark. It is important that such devices have a failsafe mechanism and shut off after a certain time, to prevent ongoing operation. 72 CHAPTER 2. ARTICLES • Combination and escalation devices - many sound and/or electrical collars have combination or escalation systems. A combination system is one that (for example) uses both sound and spray together. An escalation device is one that uses quiet sounds, or low levels of output, rising gradually until barking ceases. Escalation devices are effective since they “reward”the dog for stopping sooner by not having “all-or-nothing”action, so the dog can learn to react by stopping before much happens. • Albanian - ham ham • Arabic - hau hau; how how ( هو,)هو • Armenian -haf haf • Basque - au au; txau txau (small dogs); zaunk zaunk (large dogs); jau jau (old dogs) • Balinese - kong, kong • Belgium- woef, woef; blaf, blaf; waf, waf (large dogs) Keff, keff; Wuff, Wuff (small dogs) • Bengali - gheu, gheu; bhao, bhao Note: • Various bark collars have been both praised, and criticised, and some are considered inhumane by various people and groups. Electrical devices especially come under criticism by people who consider them torturous and akin to electrocution. However most Societies for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals agree that in a last resort even an electric collar is better than euthanasia if it comes to an ultimatum, for a stubborn dog that will not stop any other way. It is generally agreed that understanding the communication and retraining by reward is the most effective and most humane way. • Bulgarian - bau-bau (бау-бау); jaff, jaff (джафджаф) • Burmese - woke, woke • Catalan - bau, bau; bub, bub • Chinese, Cantonese - wow, wow (汪汪) • Chinese, Mandarin - wang, wang (汪汪) • Croatian - vau, vau • Czech - haf, haf; štěk (the bark itself) • Danish - vov, vuf Surgical debarking • Dutch - blaf, blaf; kef, kef; waf, waf; woef, woef Main article: Surgical debarking • Esperanto - boj, boj • Estonian - auh, auh The controversial surgical procedure known as 'debarking' is a veterinary procedure for modifying the voice box so that a barking dog will make a significantly reduced noise. It is considered a last resort by some owners, on the basis that it is better than euthanasia, seizure, or legal problems if the matter has proven incapable of being reliably corrected any other way. Debarking is illegal in many European states and opposed by animal welfare organizations. 2.4.4 Representation Woof is the conventional representation in the English language of the barking of a dog. As with other examples of onomatopoeia or imitative sounds, other cultures “hear”the dog's barks differently and represent them in their own ways. Some of the equivalents of “woof”in other European and Asian languages are as follows: • English - woof woof; ruff ruff; arf arf (large dogs and also the sound of sea lions); yap yap; yip yip (small dogs), bow wow • Afrikaans - blaf blaf; woef woef; keff keff (small dogs) • Finnish - hau, hau; vuh, vuh; rauf, rauf • French - waouh, waouh; ouah, ouah; ouaf, ouaf; vaf, vaf; wouf, wouf; wouaf, wouaf; jappe jappe • German - wuff, wuff; wau, wau; • Greek - ghav, ghav (γαβ, γαβ / γαυ, γαυ), bhav, bhav (βαβ, βαβ) • Hebrew - hav, hav; hau, hau • Hindi - bho.n, bho.n (भों भों) • Hungarian - vau, vau • Icelandic - voff, voff • Indonesian - guk, guk • Irish - amh, amh • Italian - bau, bau • Japanese - wan-wan (ワンワン); kyan-kyan (キャ ンキャン)* [15] • Korean - meong, meong ( [mʌŋmʌŋ]) , pronounced 2.4. BARK (SOUND) • kurdi - hau hau ()حەو حەو 73 2.4.6 Naturally “barkless”dog breeds • Latvian - vau, vau • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • Compared to most domestic dogs, the bark of a dingo is short and monosyllabic. During observations, the barking Lithuanian - au, au of Australian dingoes was shown to have a relatively small Macedonian - av, av variability; sub-groups of bark types, common among doMalay - gong, gong (“menggonggong”means bark- mestic dogs, could not be found. Furthermore, only 5% of the observed vocalisations were made up of barking. ing) Australian dingoes bark only in swooshing noises or in a mixture atonal/tonal. Also, barking is almost exclusively Marathi - bhu, bhu (भू भू) used for giving warnings. Warn-barking in a homotypNorwegian - voff, voff or boff ical sequence and a kind of “warn-howling”in a heterotypical sequence has also been observed. The barkPersian - haap, haap ( هاپ،)هاپ howling starts with several barks and then fades into a risPhilippines - Aw Aw, Aw ing and ebbing howl and is probably, similarly to coughing, used to warn the puppies and members of the pack. Polish - hau, hau Additionally, dingoes emit a sort of “wailing”sound, Portuguese - au, au; ão-ão (nasal diphthong); béu- which they mostly use when approaching a water hole, probably to warn already present dingoes.* [16] Accordbéu (toddler language); cain-cain (whining) ing to the present state of knowledge, it is not possible to Romanian - ham, ham; hau, hau get Australian dingoes to bark more frequently by makRussian - gav, gav (гав-гав); tyav, tyav (тяв-тяв, ing them associate with other domestic dogs. However, Alfred Brehm reported a dingo that completely learned small dogs) the more“typical”form of barking and knew how to use it, while its brother did not.* [17] Whether dingoes bark Serbian - av, av or bark-howl less frequently in general is not sure.* [18] Sinhala - බුඃ බුඃ buh, buh The now extinct Hare Indian dog of northern Canada was Slovak - haf, haf; hau, hau not known to bark in its native homeland, though puppies born in Europe learned how to imitate the barking Slovene - hov, hov of other dogs.* [19] When hurt or afraid, it howled like a wolf, and when curious, it made a sound described as a Spanish - guau-guau; gua, gua; jau, jau growl building up to a howl.* [20] Swedish - voff, voff; vov, vov; bjäbb, bjäbb The Basenji of central Africa produces an unusual yodelTagalog - aw, aw; baw, baw like sound, due to its unusually shaped larynx.* [21] This trait also gives the Basenji the nickname“Barkless Dog.” Tamil - வள் வள் - wal wal;லொள் லொள் - lol lol * [22] ;வௌ வௌ - wow wow • Thai - โฮ่ง โฮ่ง (pronounced [hôŋhôŋ]); บ๊อก บ๊อก (pronounced [bɔ́ kbɔ́ k]) 2.4.7 Barking in other animals • Turkish - hev hev; hav, hav Many animals communicate via various vocalizations. • Ukrainian - гав, гав (hau, hau); дзяв, дзяв (dzyau, While there is not a precise, consistent and functional acoustic definition for barking, researchers may clasdzyau) sify barks according to several criteria.* [23] University • Urdu - bow bow of Massachusetts Amherst researchers identified volume, pitch, tonality, noise, abrupt onset and pulse duration • Vietnamese - gâu gâu; ẳng ẳng are amongst the criteria that can be used to define a • Welsh - wff, wff bark.* [24] • Tamazight- hav hav; haw haw 2.4.5 Breeds The Huntaway is a working dog that has been selectively bred to drive stock (usually sheep), by using its voice. It was bred in New Zealand, and is still bred based on ability rather than appearance or lineage. Besides dogs and wolves, other canines like coyotes and jackals can bark. Their barks are quite similar to those of wolves and dogs. The bark of a dingo is short and monosyllabic. The warning bark of a fox sounds much like a dog's, but generally the vocalisation of foxes is higher and more drawn out than barks of other canids. There are also non-canine species with vocalizations that 74 CHAPTER 2. ARTICLES could be described as barking. Because the Muntjac's [9] Coppinger, R; M. Feinstein. "'Hark! Hark! The dogs do bark...' and bark and bark”. Smithsonian (21): 119–128. alarm call resembles a dog's bark, they are sometimes known as Barking Deer. Eared seals are also known to [10] Feddersen-Petersen, D.U. (2000).“Vocalization of Eurobark. Prairie dogs employ a complex form of commupean wolves (Canus lupus lupus L.) and various dog breeds nication that involves barks and rhythmic chirps.* [25] A (Canus lupus f., fam.)". Arch. Tierz (Kiel, Germany: Inwide variety of bird species produce vocalizations that institut für Haustierkunde, Christian-Albrechts-University) clude the canonical features of barking, especially when 4: 387–397. avoiding predators.* [24] Some primate species, notably [11] Pongrácz, P; Molnár, C.; Miklósi, Á.; Csányi, V. (2005). gorillas, can and do vocalize in short barks. “Human Listeners Are Able to Classify Dog (Canis 2.4.8 See also • Animal communication • Debarking • Dog communication • Dog training • Growling 2.4.9 References [1] Schassburger, R.M. (1987). “Wolf vocalization: An integrated model of structure, motivation, and ontogeny”. In H. Frank. Man and Wolf. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Dr. W. Junk. [2] Coscia, E.M. “Ontogeny of timber wolf vocalizations: Acoustic properties and behavioral contexts.”. Ph.D. Dissertation. (Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada: Dalhousie University). [3] Fedderson-Peterson, D.U. (2000). “Vocalization of European wolves (Canus lupus lupus L.) and various dog breeds (Canus lupus f., fam.)". Arch. Tierz (Kiel, Germany: Institut für Haustierkunde, Christian-AlbrechtsUniversity) 4: 387–397. familiaris) Barks Recorded in Different Situations”. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2 119: 136–144. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.136. [12] Yin, S (2002). “A New Perspective on Barking in Dogs (Canis familiaris)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2 116: 189–193. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.116.2.189. [13] Juarbe-D'az, Soraya V; and Houpt, Katherine A.“Calming 'Nuisance-Barking' Dogs. [Web links]". myeducationresearch.org, The Pierian Press, 3 May 1996. Online. Internet. 18 May 1743. Retrieved 30 November 2010. [14] Richard Murray and Helen Penridge. Dogs in the Urban Environment. Chiron Media 1992, ISBN 0-646-07157-2, pp. 21–22. [15] Rikaichan Japanese-English dictionary extension, version 2.01 [16] Laurie Corbett (2004).“Dingo”. Canids: Foxes, Wolves, Jackals and Dogs. International Union for Conservation of Nature and Natural Resources. Retrieved 8 April 2009. [17] Brehms Tierleben (in German). Leipzig, Wien: Bibliographisches Institut. 1900. pp. 82–85. [18] Feddersen-Petersen, Dorit Urd (2008). Ausdrucksverhalten beim Hund (in German). Stuttgart: Franckh-Kosmos Verlags-GmbH & Co. KG. ISBN 978-3-440-09863-9. [4] Coppinger, R.; M. Feinstein (1991). "'Hark! Hark! The dogs do bark...' and bark and hark.”. Smithsonian 21: 119–128. [19] The Gardens and Menagerie of the Zoological Society, Published, with the Sanction of the Council, Under the Superintendence of the Secretary and Vice-secretary of the Society, by Edward Turner Bennett, Zoological Society of London, William Harvey, Illustrated by John Jackson, William Harvey, G. B., S. S., Thomas Williams, Robert Edward Branston, George Thomas Wright. Published by Printed by C. Whittingham, 1830. [5] Belyaev, D.K.; I.Z. Plyusnina, L.N. Trut (1984). “Domestication in the silver fox (Vulpus fulvus desm.) changes in physiological boundaries of the sensitive period of primary socialization”. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 13 (4): 359–370. doi:10.1016/01681591(85)90015-2. [20] Fauna Boreali-americana, Or, The Zoology of the Northern Parts of British America: Containing Descriptions of the Objects of Natural History Collected on the Late Northern Land Expeditions, Under Command of Captain Sir John Franklin, R.N. By John Richardson, William Swainson, William Kirby, published by J. Murray, 1829. [6] Fox, M.W. (1986). Saunders, W.B., ed. “The influence of domestication upon behavior of animals”. Abnormal Behaviour in Animals (Philadelphia): 179–187. [21] Adapted from the book “Why Pandas Do Handstands,” 2006, by Augustus Brown. [22] BCOA African Stock Project - 1945 Letter from Africa [7] Dechambre, E. (1949). “La theorie de la foetalisation et la formation des races de chiens et de porcs”. Mammalia (in French) 13: 129–137. doi:10.1515/mamm.1949.13.3.129. [23] Not Only Dogs, But Deer, Monkeys And Birds Bark To Deal With Conflict. Science Daily. 15 July 2009. [8] Fox, M.W. (1971). “Behavior of wolves and related canids”. Malabar, FL. [25] Walker, Matt. Burrowing US prairie dogs use complex language. BBC Earth News. 2 February 2010. [24] Lord, Kathryn., Feinstein, Mark., Coppinger, Raymond. Barking and mobbing. Behavioral Processes. 2009. 2.5. BITE INHIBITION 2.4.10 External links (relationship building) • "Is the Bark Worse Than the Bite?". Jennifer K. Rudolph, BS, and Lawrence Myers, DVM, MS, PhD, Veterinary Forum, 1994 75 rior animal as an act of submission. The superior animal could, in theory, kill the other immediately, but he instead shows mercy as the alpha. Submission was thought to reduce losses for an animal that knows it cannot challenge the other.* [8] A few years later, this idea was challenged by Rudolf Schenkel, who suggests that, quite contrary to Lorenzʼ • “Article from ASPCA's Virtual Pet Behaviorist on s beliefs, the inferior dog is the one with his jaws open working with barking problems” near to the superiorʼs neck. The superior canine remains growling and his posture is erect, as though to prepare for • Sound of foxes barking. Wav file. an attack. Schenkel suggests that the bite inhibition in this instance is shown by the inferior to show that he does not dare to bite the superior.* [9] 2.5 Bite inhibition Bite inhibition, sometimes referred to as a soft mouth (a term which also has a distinct meaning), is a behavior in carnivorans (dogs, cats,* [1] etc.) whereby the animal learns to moderate the strength of its bite. It is an important factor in the socialization of pets.* [2] 2.5.2 Chemicals Involved in Aggression ity to monitor the strength of their bites. In addition to its role in domestication, bite inhibition is also a significant part of the development of dominance hierarchy in wild animals such as wolves.* [4] However, while short-term increases in GCs can be beneficial under stress, long-term increases are harmful to health, as GCs contribute to immune and reproductive system suppression, as well as loss in muscle mass.* [11] Therefore, being a dominant individual in the pack has a high cost(and high benefit), while accepting subordination is low cost-low benefit.* [12] Testosterone has a major effect on aggression in animals. Dogs with excess testosterone are found to act out violently, and are far less likely to practice bite inhibition, * Bite inhibition is typically learned as part of juvenile play especially without proper training. [10] behaviors, when the animal is still in the company of its In observations of a wild population of gray wolves, or mother and siblings: by biting each other during play, the canis lupus, levels of adrenal glucocorticoid(GCs) were young animals learn that biting a companion too strongly found to be elevated in dominant wolves. GCs affect the leads to the abrupt termination of play activities.* [3] stress responses in vertebrates, redirecting energy from Bite inhibition is an important factor in the socialization systems such as the digestive and reproductive to the of pets because many breeds do not innately have the abil- senses and heart to eliminate immediate threats. 2.5.1 Evolution of Bite Inhibition in Modern Dogs Modern dogs learn bite inhibition for the same reason that their ancestors, the wolves, did: in order to establish an effective dominance hierarchy. Dominance hierarchy is a term which describes the "pecking order" in groups of animals.* [5] It allows for tranquility in large groups when each individual knows its place. Dominance hierarchies are formed in groups of canines through intense displays of aggression.* [6] However, this type of vying for dominance has only been observed in forced groupings of captive wolves.* [7] In the wild, this trend is less common, as wolves tend to group off into family units instead of unrelated adults. Therefore, the alpha male and alpha female would simply be the parents, and the offspring would submit readily. Bite inhibition, then, naturally occurs as the pups learn not to bite their siblings and parents too hard. Catecholamines, such as epinephrine, or adrenaline, norepinephrine, and dopamine, also have effects on aggression. An increase in catecholamines assist with the body's fight-or-flight response by increasing blood flow to the muscles, decreasing pain sensitivity, and improving attention. Dogs with higher levels of these chemicals tend to be more aggressive, because they are more ready to fight.* [13] 2.5.3 Training Bite inhibition is typically learned as part of juvenile play behaviors, when the animal is still in the company of its mother and siblings: by biting each other during play, the young animals learn that biting a companion too strongly leads to the abrupt termination of play activities. This behavior is crucial later in life, as well, when dogs need Lorenz vs. Schenkel: Interpreting Canine Aggres- to maintain the carefully constructed dominance hierarchies. Therefore, a useful method for training a puppy or sion dog to monitor the strength of its bite would simply be Austrian scientist Konrad Lorenz explains that the infe- to ignore the dog immediately after the incident occurs. rior animal shows its most vulnerable part to the supe- This way, the dog learns that harmful biting will lead to 76 CHAPTER 2. ARTICLES punishment. A dogʼ s first instinct to unpleasant stimulus is not a bite. A dog will use several techniques to stop what he perceives as a threat before he resorts to biting. Therefore, it is important to avoid suppressing important canine communications such as growling and snarling. If a dog learns that a growl is an inappropriate response to a threat, then humans may be encountered with an unexpected bite when they accidentally, for example, step on the dogʼs tail. Even a dog that would never bite out of anger can snap when met with a painful or threatening stimulus, so training in bite inhibition can be useful to keep them from accidentally hurting another dog or human.* [14] [14] Miller, Pat. “Teaching Bite Inhibition”. The Whole Dog Journal. Belvoir Media Group. Retrieved 12 November 2014. 2.6 BowLingual BowLingual (バウリンガル), or“Bow-Lingual”as the North American version is spelled, is a computer-based dog-to-human language translation device developed by Japanese toy company Takara and first sold in Japan in 2002. Versions for South Korea and the United States were launched in 2003. The device was named by Time magazine as a “Best Invention of 2002.”The inventors of BowLingual, Keita Satoh, Dr. Matsumi Suzuki and 2.5.4 References Dr. Norio Kogure were awarded the Ig Nobel Prize for “promoting peace and harmony between the species.” [1] Domestic Animal Behavior (4th edition) by Katherine A. Houpt, Wiley-Blackwell Publications, 2005 The device is presented as a “translator”but has been [2] Before & After Getting Your Puppy: The Positive Ap- called an “emotion analyzer”. It is said to use technolproach to Raising a Happy, Healthy, & Well-Behaved Dog ogy to categorize dog barks into one of six standardized emotional categories. BowLingual also provides a phrase by Ian Dunbar, New World Library, 2004 which is representative of that emotion. The product in[3] The Everything Dog Obedience Book: from bad dog to structions clearly state that these phrases “are for entergood dog -- a step-by-step guide to curbing misbehavior tainment purposes only”and are not meant to be accurate by Jennifer Bridwell, F+W Publications, 2007 translations of each bark. [4] Lindsay, Steven R. (2001). Handbook of Applied Dog Behavior and Training, Volume 2: Etiology and assessment of behavior problems. Iowa State University Press. 2.6.1 Features [5] Ehrlich, Paul; Dobkin, David; Wheye, Darryl. “Dominance Hierarchies”. web.stanford.edu. Retrieved 28 October 2014. [6] [7] [8] [9] [10] [11] BowLingual has several functions which include dog training tips, a “Bow Wow Diary,”tips on understanding a dog's body language, a medical checklist and a Houpt, Katherine A. (2005). Domestic Animal Behavior. home alone bark recording function. The device conBlackwell Publishing. sists of a hand-held receiver that contains a LCD inforMech, L. David. “Alpha status, dominance, and division mation screen and functions as the controller and a wireof labor in wolf packs”. nrc research press. less microphone-transmitter which is attached to the dog's Lorenz, Konrad (2002). King Solomon's Ring. Routledge. collar. Retrieved 28 October 2014. When a dog barks, the microphone records and transmits Schenkel, Rudolf. Submission: Its Functions and Features the sound to the hand-held unit for computer analysis by a database with thousands of dog barks pre-recorded into in Wolf and Dog. Retrieved 28 October 2014. it. The unit then categorizes the bark into one of six disO'Heare, James. “The Effects of Spaying and Neutering tinct dog emotions (happy, sad, frustrated, on-guard, ason Canine Behavior”. Association of Animal Behavior sertive, needy) and displays the corresponding emotion Professionals. James O'Heare. Retrieved 12 November on the screen. After displaying the emotion, BowLingual 2014. then displays a phrase which has been categorized to fit Creel, Scott. “Social Dominance and Stress Hormones” within the range of each emotion. . Science Direct. Elsevier Ltd. Retrieved 12 November 2014. [12] Creel, Scott; Sands, Jennifer.“Social dominance, aggression and faecal glucocorticoid levels in a wild population of wolves, Canis lupus”. Science Direct. Elsevier Ltd. Retrieved 12 November 2014. 2.6.2 Versions Regional versionals of BowLingual have been released in Japan, South Korea, the US and Canada. The versions for [13] Haller, J; Makara, G.B.; Kruk, M.R. “Catecholaminer- South Korean South Korea, the US and Canada have difgic involvement in the control of aggression: hormones, ferent modifications in comparison to the Japanese verthe peripheral sympathetic, and central noradrenergic sys- sion. In May 2003, at the request of the Japan Foreign tems”. Science Direct. Elsevier Ltd. Retrieved 12 Novem- Ministry, Takara provided Japanese Prime Minister Juber 2014. nichiro Koizumi with two prototypes of the English ver- 2.7. CANINE GOOD CITIZEN 77 sion of BowLingual several months before it had been re- 2.6.6 References leased in North America. Koizumi then presented these to Russian President Vladimir Putin, for each of his dogs [1] Gadget's bark is bigger than its hype / Vet puts 'bark translator' to the test. Verdict: nothing more than $120 curios(Tosca, a standard Poodle, and Connie, a Labrador Reity triever), at ceremonies celebrating the 300th anniversary of St. Petersburg. [2] Molnar et al. (2008) Classification of dog barks: a machine learning approach. Animal Cognition, 11, 389-400 2.6.3 Effectiveness BowLingual uses customized voice-print analysis technology which has been adapted for dog barks. The accuracy of this product can be affected by varying conditions and situations. Sound interference can occur when the wireless collar-microphone picks up noises made by chain collars and collars with dog tags attached. As a result, the dog owner may believe that the device is malfunctioning and not registering the dog bark correctly. 2.7 Canine Good Citizen In windy conditions, the microphone will sometimes interpret a gust of wind as a bark. Electrical equipment and certain radio signals may trigger false readouts. Due to improvements with the US and Canadian versions of the products, these problems are more common with the Japanese and South Korean versions. One reviewer of the product, vet Sophia Yin stated “it's not very useful because the translations aren't trustworthy Canine Good Citizen dog tag. and most don't make sense.”* [1] Csaba Molnar and colleagues at Eotvos Lorand University, Budapest, Hungary proved by computer-based machine learning algorythms that there are consistent differences in the acoustics of dog barks according to the behavioural context and individuals.* [2] The Canine Good Citizen (CGC) program, established in 1989, is an American Kennel Club program to promote responsible dog ownership and to encourage the training of well-mannered dogs. A dog and handler team must take a short behavioral evaluation of less than half an hour; dogs who pass the evaluation earn the Canine Good Citizen certificate, which many people represent after the 2.6.4 Related products dog's name, abbreviating it as CGC; for example,“Fido, In 2003 Takara launched a follow-up product for cats CGC”. called Meowlingual (ミャウリンガル). It functioned The evaluation consists of ten objectives. All items must similarly to BowLingual; however, it did not use the wire- be completed satisfactorily or the team fails. Test items less microphone system. Instead, the microphone was include: contained in the main hand-held unit so that the user had to be close enough to, in effect, “interview”the cat. • Accepting a friendly stranger. Without the wireless component, Meowlingual was considerably cheaper than BowLingual. Meowlingual was • Sitting politely for petting. never launched in the U.S. or any other countries, so only Japanese language versions exist. • Allowing basic grooming procedures. 2.6.5 External links • Time Magazine Best Inventions 2002, Bowlingual • BowLingual Web page (archived) • MeowLingual Web page (archived) • Bowlingual presented by Japan prime minister to Russian president • Critical review • Walking on a loose lead. • Walking through a crowd. • Sitting and lying down on command and staying in place. • Coming when called. • Reacting appropriately to another dog. • Reacting appropriately to distractions. 78 CHAPTER 2. ARTICLES • Calmly enduring supervised separation from the owner. Evaluators sometimes combine elements during the actual test. If all ten objectives are met, the handler can apply for a certificate and special dog tag from the AKC stating that the dog has earned the CGC. Dogs do not have to be registered with the AKC to earn a CGC, nor do they have to be purebred or, in fact, registered with any canine organization. The goal is to promote good citizenship for all dogs. Since its inception, the CGC program has become the model for similar programs around the world, is the back- Leather buckle collar with traditional buckle. bone of other exams, such as those given for therapy dogs, and is a good starting point for more advanced dog trainsays you should be able to get two fingers underneath ing. the collar.* [5] 2.7.1 External links • AKC Canine Good Citizen section 2.8 Dog collar For the detachable white collar worn by Christian clergy, see clerical collar. A dog collar is a piece of material put around the neck of a dog. A collar may be used for control, identification, fashion, or other purposes. Identification tags and medical information are often placed on dog collars.* [1] Collars are also useful for controlling a dog manually, as they provide a handle for grabbing. Collars are often used in conjunction with a leash, and a common alternative to a dog collar is a dog harness. Dog collars are the most Nylon quick-release buckle collar with identification and medical common form of directing and teaching dogs.* [2] tags. Dog collar is also an informal term for the clerical collar used by Anglican vicars and other clergy.* [3] 2.8.1 Basic collars • Buckle collars, also called flat collars, are usually made of nylon webbing* [4] or leather (less common materials can include polyester, hemp, or metal) with a buckle similar to a belt buckle, or a quick-release buckle, either of which holds the collar loosely around the dog's neck. Identification is commonly attached to such a collar; it also comes with a loop to which a leash can be fastened. This is the most standard collar for dogs. A flat collar should fit comfortably tight on your dog's neck. It should not be so tight as to choke your dog nor so loose that they can slip out of it. The rule of thumb • Flea collars are impregnated with chemicals that repel fleas.* [6] They are usually a supplementary collar, worn in addition to the conventional buckle collar. • Elizabethan collars, shaped like a truncated cone, can be fitted on a dog to prevent it from scratching a wound on its head or neck or licking a wound or infection on its body.* [7] • Break-away collars look similar to buckle collars, but have a safety mechanism installed that allows the dog to break free of the collar if excessive force is applied. These collars are useful in situations where a non-quick release collar could get snagged and strangle the dog.* [8] 2.8. DOG COLLAR 79 • Safety Stretch Collars an elastic panel in the sturdy nylon collar allows escape from potential strangulation dangers such as branches, fences, gates and other dogs. Unlike breakaways a stretch collar acts like a traditional static collar when clipped with a leash. • Stud collars are leather collars fitted with dulled points and/or metal studs that traditionally prevented another animal from biting the dog's neck. This type of collar dates back to ancient Greece, when sheepdogs were given nail-studded collars to protect them from wolves.* [9] In modern societies, stud collars are more commonly considered a fashion accessory. • Painted collars are leather collars with a pattern applied with safe water-resistant paint. Usually the paint is applied manually. These collars are more Martingale Collar with Chain Loop; martingale collars also come expensive than others because of handiwork. with a fabric loop instead of chain as well as optional buckles on both styles. • Oilcloth collars are made of vinyl woven with cotton. They are long-lasting, water resistant and stain Martingale collar resistant. The surface of the material can be wiped clean. These collars are sturdy and hard to tear. Martingale collars are recommended for Sighthounds because their heads are smaller than their necks and they can • Spiked collars are made of nylon or leather mate- often slip out of standard collars. They can, however, be rial and decorated with metal spikes. Commonly, used for any breed of dog. Their no-slip feature has made the spikes are hand-set and tightly riveted for extra them a safety standard at many kennels and animal shelsecurity. Spikes prevent other animals from biting ters. A martingale collar has 2 loops; the smaller loop the dog's neck and serve as fancy accessory. is the “control loop”that tightens the larger loop when pulled to prevent dogs from slipping out of the collar. A • Reflective collars are made with 3M reflective tape correctly adjusted martingale does not constrict the dog's that ensures the dog will be seen at night by ap- neck when pulled taut. proaching vehicles. These collars are usually made with nylon webbing and can provide reflection up to Head halters 1,000 feet in the dark.* [10] 2.8.2 Training collars Head halters, sold under the brand names “Comfort Trainer”,Halti or Gentle Leader or Snoot Loop, are similar in design to a halter for a horse. This device fastens around the back of the neck and over the top of the muzzle, giving more control over a dog's direction and the intensity of pulling on a leash than collars that fit strictly around the neck. Pressure on this type of collar pulls the dog's head towards the handler. These type of collars can stop a strong dog pulling an owner in an unsafe direction. They are also good for dogs that pull as the pressure will no longer be directly on their wind pipe.* [11] Several types of collars are used for the purposes of training dogs, though sometimes a collar is not used at all (such as in the case of dog agility training, where a collar could get caught on equipment and strangle the dog). Each training collar has its own set of advantages and disadvantages (briefly outlined below) which trainers might consider before using a select one. Training collars are typically used for training only and not left on the dog's neck all the time, as some collars can be harmful or danControversy Supporters of the head halter say that it gerous if left on a dog unsupervised. enables the handler to control the dog's head, and makes the dog unable to pull using its full strength. It is especially useful with reactive dogs, when control of the dog's Flat collars head can be a safety issue. Most dogs are trained on leash using a buckle or quick- Those who do not recommend use of the head halter say release collar. that some dogs find it unnatural and uncomfortable. If the 80 CHAPTER 2. ARTICLES Main article: Dog § Biology Most lighted collars utilize one or more light emitting diodes for the light source and can be of virtually any color, although red and blue are most common. Power is provided by one or more batteries, most common types being AAA and lithium coin cells to minimize the added weight to the collar. Flotation collar A flotation collar (or buoyant collar) is a buoyancy aid designed for dogs. Although it is not designed to be used as a life preserver or life jacket, it can provide additional buoyant support for the head of a dog when in the water. It is often used in canine hydrotherapy services to assist in the rehabilitation of injured dogs. The collar may be constructed of closed cell foam material that is inherently buoyant or be of a type that is inflated with air. Aversive collars The halter-style collar controls the dog's head but does not restrict its ability to pant, drink, or grasp objects. collar is too tight, it may dig too deeply into the skin or the strap around the muzzle may push into the dog's eyes. Cranial injury is a possible result from improper use of the head halter; if a dog is jerked suddenly by the leash attached to the head halter, the dog's neck is pulled sharply to the side, which might result in neck injury (though this can be true of all collars). If the nose strap is fitted too tightly, the hair on the muzzle can also be rubbed off, or the dog might paw and scratch at its face, causing injuries ranging from mere bare skin to severe abrasions. Aversive collars use discomfort or pain to cause a dog to stop doing unwanted behaviors.* [12] The use of aversive collars is controversial, with many humane and veterinary organizations recommending against them.* [13]* [14]* [15] • Shock collars (or training collars, remote training collars, e-collars, electronic collars* [16] and hunting collars) are electronic training aids developed to deliver an electrical signal, vibration, tone, through contact points attached to a dog collar.* [17]* [18] Shock collars are illegal in Wales* [19] and many other countries. Some research indicates that training using positive reinforcement achieves faster results than the same behavior trained using shock collars.* [20] Wolf collars Wolf collars or protection collars are metal collars fitted with large spikes radiating away from the dog, usually worn by dogs protecting livestock in case they are attacked by wolves or other predators. Such collars protect the neck of a dog from direct attack. It is rare to see these collars being used in modern societies. Lighted collar A lighted collar (or collar light, dog light) is a collar that emits light in order to make a dog more visible in the dark Prong collar; the looped chain limits how tightly the collar can pull in the same way that a Martingale functions. to their owners and more importantly, nearby motorists. It should be noted that it is not designed to help a dog see at night, as it is well documented that dogs have very good • Prong collars are a series of chain links with vision in low light conditions. blunted open ends turned towards the dog's neck. 2.8. DOG COLLAR The design of the prong collar is such that it has a limited circumference unlike choke chains which do not have a limit on how far they can constrict on a dog's neck. The limited traction of the martingale chain combined with the angle of the prongs prevents the prongs moving close enough to pinch. The collar is designed to prevent the dog from pulling by applying pressure at each point against the dog's neck. Prong collars must never be turned inside out (with the prongs facing away from the dog's skin), as this may cause injury against the body and head.* [21] Plastic tips are occasionally placed on the ends of the prongs to protect against tufts forming in the fur or, in the case of low quality manufactured collars with rough chisel cut ends, irritating the skin. Like the choke chain, the prong collar is placed high on the dog's neck, just behind the ears, at the most sensitive point.* [22] Some dogs can free themselves from prong collars with large wire looped sides by shaking their head so that the links pop out, so some trainers have come to use a second collar (usually an oversize check chain) in addition to the prong collar so when this happens the dog does not run loose. • Force collars are leather with metal prongs staggered along the inside; similar to a prong collar. 81 snap, or correction. This is supposed to correct a dog's unwanted behavior, such as leaving the“heel” position. Pulling harder or longer on the choke chain presses on the dog's esophagus and restricts breathing. Cesar Milan's“Illusion collar”is a choke collar wrapped in a buckle collar.* [24] • Fur saver collars are a kind of choke chains that provide less effect on the dog's hair, thus not damaging it. Fur saver collar can be used both for long and short-haired breeds without making any harm to the dog's fur. It can be used for training and daily life as well. 2.8.3 See also • Collar • Muzzle (device) • Shock collar 2.8.4 References [1] Clayden, Paul, ed. (2011-05-25). The Dog Law Handbook (2nd ed.). London: Sweet & Maxwell. p. 35. ISBN 978-0-414-04818-8. [2] Hodgson, Sarah (2006). Teach Yourself Visually Dog Training. Wiley Default. ISBN 0-471-74989-3. [3] “Dog collar clergy 'risk attack'". BBC News. 7 October 2007. Retrieved 30 December 2011. [4] Ogburn, Philip; Crouse, Stephanie, Martin, Frank, Houpt, Katherine (1 December 1998). “Comparison of behavioral and physiological responses of dogs wearing two different types of collars”. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 61 (2): 133–142. doi:10.1016/S01681591(98)00113-0. [5] http://www.humanesociety.org/animals/dogs/tips/ collars.html. Missing or empty |title= (help) Choke chain, showing how the chain pulls through the loop at one end. • Choke chains (also called choke collars or slip chains) are a length of chain with rings at either end such that the collar can be formed into a loop that slips over the dogs head and rests around the top of the dog's neck, just behind the ears.* [23] When the leash is attached to the dead ring, the collar does not constrict on the dog's neck. When the leash is attached to the live ring, the chain slips (adjusts) tighter when pulled and slips looser when tension is released. Training with this leash involves a quick jerk with an immediate release, called a leash pop, [6] Cronce, P. C.; Alden, H. S. (11 November 1968). “Flea-Collar Dermatitis”. JAMA: the Journal of the American Medical Association 206 (7): 1563–1564. doi:10.1001/jama.1968.03150070101023. [7] Swaim, Steven F.; Renberg, Walter C.; Shike, Kathy M. (2010-12-15). Small Animal Bandaging, Casting, and Splinting Techniques. Ames, Iowa: Wiley-Blackwell. ISBN 978-0-8138-1962-4. [8] Monteiro, Melanie (2009). Safe Dog Handbook: A Complete Guide to Protecting Your Pooch, Indoors and Out. Beverly, Mass.: Quarry Books. p. 96. ISBN 978-159253-519-4. [9] http://www.dogcollarsboutique.com/ A-History-of-Dog-Collars-sp-17.html 82 CHAPTER 2. ARTICLES [10] http://www.shopmimigreen.com/ Engraved-Buckle-Personalized-Dog-Collars/ Reflective-Dog-Collar-with-Engraved-Personalized-Name-Plate. html [11] https://www.sfspca.org/sites/default/files/dog_ head-halters-and-harnesses.pdf [12] Humane Society. “Dog Collars: Aversive Collars”. Retrieved 2014-08-01. [13] Humane Society. “Dog Collars: Aversive Collars”. Retrieved 2014-08-01. [14] American Veterinary Society of Animal Behavior. “AVSAB Position Statement The Use of Punishment for Behavior Modification in Animals”. Retrieved 2014-0801. [15] San Francisco SPCA.“Trade in Your Pronged Dog Collar”. Retrieved 2014-08-01. Dogs communicating that their intentions are not aggression but play - a form of metacommunication The study of animal communication ̶sometimes called zoosemiotics (defined as the study of sign communication or semiosis in animals; distinguishable from [17] “Electric Dog Collars Hazardous”. The Milwaukee Jouranthroposemiotics, the study of human communication) nal. 20 April 1977. ̶has played an important part in ethology, sociobiology, [18] “Shocking dog collars recalled”. St. Petersburg Times. and the study of animal cognition. [16] “Electronic dog collars in stomach of alligator solve Florida mystery”. Toledo Blade. August 29, 1995. May 20, 1976. [19] “Ogmore illegal shock collar dog owner gets £2,000 fine” . BBC News. 18 July 2011. 2.9.1 Dog-human communication [20] http://blog.smartanimaltraining.com/2013/07/31/ Dogs were the first species to live so closely to hunew-findings-on-shock-collars-why-the-uk-wants-to-ban-them/ mans, starting between 35,000 and 100,000 years ago. vv They have developed a complex way of communicat[21] “Herm Sprenger Prong Collar Covering Caps”. Luvmydog.co.uk. Retrieved 30 December 2011. [22] “How to fit a Prong Collar”. Leerburg. Retrieved 30 December 2011. [23] “Dictionary of Dog Collar Terms”. bigdogboutique.com. Retrieved 12 August 2012. [24] “Illusion collar”. Retrieved 2014-08-01. 2.8.5 External links • A History of Dog Collars 2.9 Dog communication Dog communication is any transfer of information on the part of one or more dogs that has an effect on the current or future behaviour of the dog(s), or another animal. Dog communication occurs in a variety of forms and is part of the foundation of dog social behavior. Dogs use all the major sensory modalities to communicate, including visual (e.g. movements of their bodies and limbs), auditory (vocalizations), tactile (touch) and gustatory communication (scents, pheromones and taste). ing with humans and forming relationships, giving rise to the English phrase “man's best friend”.* [1] There are many different definitions of communication.* [2] Communication between dogs and humans can take various forms. Since dogs were first domesticated by humans, dogs have learned to adapt to the social setting of humans. Dogs have either developed, or been artificially selected for, traits and skills which allow them to live successfully with humans. These behaviours include pointing, marking, body posture and eye gaze direction.* [2] Humans communicate with dogs by using audible and tactile signals. The pointing gesture is a human-specific signal, is referential in its nature, and is a foundational building-block of human communication. Human infants acquire it weeks before the first spoken word.* [3] In 2009, a study compared the responses to a range of pointing gestures by dogs and human infants. The study showed little difference in the performance of 2-year-old children and dogs, while 3-year-old childrenʼ s performance was higher. The results also showed that all subjects were able to generalize from their previous experience to respond to relatively novel pointing gestures. These findings suggest that dogs demonstrate a similar level of performance as 2-year-old children that can be explained as a joint outcome of their evolutionary history as well as their socialization in a hu- 2.9. DOG COMMUNICATION man environment.* [4] See also: coevolution 2.9.2 83 To signal dominance, a wolf or dog stands stiff-legged and tall. The ears are held erect and forward while the tail is held vertically with the hackles (erectile hairs along the back) slightly raised. * [10]* [11] Evolution of dog-human communi- A wolf or dog will show active submission by drawing back the lips and ears, and lowering the body. The tail cation is held low or completely tucked under the body, and the may partially arch down to further display deference. There are several hypotheses about how dog-human com- back * * [10] [11] munication evolved. It is believed that the ancestral precursor of the domestic dog was a wolf-like canid. Aspects of the wolfʼs social behavior have been artificially selected over many generations to the modern domestic dogs. Wolves live and hunt as a group. There are both positive and negative consequences of group-living and it is possible that the social communicative skills of dogs arose from the social behaviors of ancestral, group-living wolves.* [5] Dogs have been domesticated by humans for thousands of years. It is possible that during this period, dogs began to mimic the behaviors of humans. Dogs have been trained by humans and have become dependent on humans. It has been suggested that this dependency trait enables them to communicate better with their owners. When dogs are more exposed to humans, their communicative skills improve.* [6] The third reason is the domestication of dog which lead to their evolution domestic characteristics such as their tameness. This might be the most prominent reason of all. The tameness of dogs might have been naturally selected when they started to live with humans or were already selected for the tameness. By living together for thousands of years, the dogʼs skill eventually evolved over time whether they are by artificial selection or natural selection.* [7] High ability of dog understanding human comes from selection against fear and aggression towards human.* [8] A wolf or dog will communicate a more intense deference through passive submissive behaviours. The animal rolls on his back, exposing the vulnerable underside and throat. The paws are drawn into the body while eye contact is avoided. The tail may be tucked in and whimpering noises may be heard. * [10]* [11] 2.9.4 Visual communication See also: Wolf body language Tail: How high or low the tail is held, in relation to how the dog's breed naturally carries its tail, and how it is moved can signify the dog's mood. When the tail is held high, it shows that the dog is alert and aware; the tail between the legs means that the dog is frightened. If the fur on the tail is also bristled, the dog is saying it is willing to defend itself or pups. If the dog does not have a tail, or it has been shortened or removed via docking, then similar actions may occur with just the hind quarters. Small, slow wags of the tail say the dog is questioning things around the environment it is in. Either it is not sure whether it should submit, the other creature is friendly, or confused about its surroundings. Large, fast wags of the tail may be a sign of a happy, excited, or an energetic dog, but can also signal aggression. Dogs communicating with their tail were illustrated in The social behaviors and their social cognition of dogs Charles Darwin's The Expression of the Emotions in Man are not yet revealed fully. There are for sure, something and Animals published in 1872. special about dogs and the relationship between dogs and humans. It is important for the researchers to study more • Examples of tail position communicating different about social skills and the social relationship of dogs and emotions in dogs. humans. The research of the evolution of dogʼ s social behaviors might help in finding the evolution of social cog•“Small dog watching a cat on a table” nition in humans. •“Dog approaching another dog with hostile intentions” 2.9.3 Dominance and submission •“Dog in a humble and affectionate frame of mind” Further information: Dog behavior •“Half-bred shepherd dog” Genetic research has indicated that domesticated dogs evolved from a now extinct wolf-like canid.* [9] Wolves primarily live in social family groups called “packs”in which they communicate in ways that can be observed in their domesticated descendants. Included in this are communications of dominance and submission.* [10] •“Dog caressing his master” Dogs are said to exhibit a left-right asymmetry of the tail when interacting with strangers, and will show the opposite, right-left motion with people and dogs they know.* [12] 84 CHAPTER 2. ARTICLES Ears: Ear position relates the dog's level of attention, and reaction, to a situation or animal. Erect ears facing forward means the dog is very attentive. They lay their ears back for the sounds surrounding them and also when in a submissive state. Dogs with drop ears, like Beagles, can't use these signals very well, as the signals first developed in wolves, whose ears are pricked. Wolf-like dogs (such as the Samoyed or Husky) will, when content and happy, often hold their ears in a horizontal position but still forward. This has been referred to as the “wolf smile”. Two dogs communicating aggression; note the teeth baring and lip curl. A dog communicating anxiety; note the white“half moon”eyes, nose licking and sideways glance. Mouth: Mouth expressions can provide information about the dog's mood. When a dog wants to be left alone, it might yawn (although yawning also might indicate sleepiness, confusion, or stress) or start licking its mouth without the presence of any food. When a dog is happy or wants to play, it might pant with lips relaxed, covering the teeth and with what sometimes appears to be a happy expression and might appear as a smile to some observers. This dog is not“smiling”but is communicating that it is defensive about its food treat. Teeth baring: When a dog's lips curl back this shows that the dog has a strong urge to bite. This is an unconscious reflex, designed to get the soft flesh of the lips away from the teeth before the dog bites, and is often misinterpreted as a way of communicating aggressive intent. For example, many dogs will curl their lips back into a “snarl”when they take a cookie or bone. A rare form of teeth baring is seen in the form as a submissive grin. This means that the dog will be submissive and friendly to the person it is grinning at. In this event the dog will also display other behavioral cues, including tail wagging and lowered posture. The dog sometimes will show a submissive grin when it has recently done something it knows its master would not like, or when it has been caught doing it. Mouth expressions that indicate aggression include the snarl, with lips retracting to expose the teeth, although some dogs also use this during play. However, some dogs will pull back their“top lips”in what looks like an aggressive way, when they are excited or happy. For example a dog prone to“smiling”may do so in greeting to a much loved owner and this should not be punished lest the dog become less affectionate. A very common form of communication as well, is for a dog to lick another dog, or a person. Dogs lick other dogs' faces and mouths when they greet each other to indicate friendliness. Dogs like to lick human skin not only for the salt from the sweat, but also as a form of greeting, such as by briefly licking a person's hand after sniffing it. Licking is also used as a social bonding analogous to primate social grooming and stroking. This can indicate intimacy. Such licking is longer and slower, as compared to the brief licking of faces during a greeting. 2.9. DOG COMMUNICATION 85 Eyes and eyebrows: While dogs do not have actual eyebrows, they do have a distinctive ridge above their eyes, and some breeds, like the Labrador Retriever, Gordon Setter, Rottweiler, Bernese Mountain Dog, German Shepherd, and Doberman have markings there. A dog's eyebrow movements usually express a similar emotion to that of a human's eyebrow movements. Raised eyebrows suggest interest, lowered brows suggest uncertainty or mild anger, and one eyebrow up suggests bewilderment. Eyes narrowed to slits indicate affection for the person or animal the dog is looking at. A dog holding its head to one side and its ears forward to focus on a sound. 2.9.5 Auditory communication Main article, including bark control training: Bark (utterance) Further information: Auditory communication in gray wolves Two dogs stamping their feet, (maybe) to gain attention. Barks: Dogs bark for many reasons, such as when perceived intruders (humans, dogs, or other animals unknown to them) approach their living space, when hearing an unfamiliar or unidentified noise, when seeing something that the dog doesn't expect to be there, or when playing. Barking also expresses such emotions as loneliness, fear, suspicion, stress, and pleasure. Playful or excited barks are often short and sharp and often made when a dog is attempting to get a person or another dog to play. Feet and legs: Although a dog's feet lack the dexterity of human hands, a dog can use them as an avenue of communication. A dog might stamp its feet, alternating its left and right front legs, while its back legs are still. This occurs when the dog is excited, wants something, or wants its owner's attention. Pointers tend to tuck one front leg Dogs generally try to avoid conflict; their vocalizations are up when they sense game nearby. part of what allows other dogs to tune into their emotions, This behavior is not communicative so much as the dog i.e., whether they're aggressive or are in a playful mood. exhibiting a fixed-action pattern called“the eye stalk.”It is also common for dogs to paw or scratch for objects they desire. Many dogs are trained to mimic a human handshake, offering a paw to a human stooping down and offering their own hand in exchange. Dogs might playfully slap each other with their paws to show gratitude toward one another. The bark of a distressed or stressed dog is high pitched, repetitive, and increases its pitch as the dog becomes more upset. For example, a dog that suffers separation anxiety may bark when left home alone. Some breeds of dogs have been bred to bark when chasing; for example, scent hounds whose handlers use the bark to follow the dog if it has run out of sight. Head: The leaning of a dog's head to the right or to the Coonhounds and Bloodhounds are good examples. Such left often indicates curiosity and/or a sound it has not barking is often called “singing”because the sound is heard before. It is also used to locate the source of the longer and more tonal. sound by adjusting the ears, so that sound waves might Some research has suggested that dogs have separate reach the ears at different times, enabling the source to barks for different animals, including dog, fox, deer, hube located. This, however, may also be a sign of recogni- man, squirrel and cat.* [13] tion to a familiar word. Growls: Growls can express aggression, a desire to play, If the dog's head is held high with its neck craning for- or simply that the dog doesn't want to participate in what's ward, it is showing interest, although, it could also mean about to happen next (being picked up for example). Most an aggressive mood if other body language is present. pet owners have therefore been urged to treat growls with A bowed head indicates submission and can be a request special attention: always consider the context of a growl for physical affection. and exercise caution. If the threat is very serious, then the 86 CHAPTER 2. ARTICLES dog will usually start off with a very low toned but strong growl that rises in tone if ignored. Howls: Howling may provide long-range communication with other dogs or owners. Howling can be used to locate another pack member, to keep strangers away, or to call the pack for hunting. Some dogs howl when they have separation anxiety. Dogs howling can also be caused by musical instruments, like harmonicas. •“What is a 'Jewish Dog'? Konrad Lorenz and the Cult of Wildness.”Boria Sax, Society and Animals, Volume 5, Number 1, 1997, pp. 3–21(19) • My Doggie Says...; Messages from Jamie by Fred Haney ISBN 0-9785515-0-8 • On Talking Terms With Dogs: Calming Signals by Turid Rugaas ISBN 0-9674796-0-6 Further information: Twilight bark 2.9.8 References Whines: Whining is a high-pitched vocalization that is often produced nasally with the mouth closed. A dog may whine when it wants something (e.g., food) wants to go outside (possibly to excrete) wants to be let off the leash (possibly to greet another dog or a person) or just wants attention. A very insistent dog may add a bark at the end of a whine, in a whine-bark, whine-bark pattern. Whimpers: A whimper or a yelp often indicates the dog is in pain or distress and is often emitted by dogs that have been bitten too hard during a play-fight. The whimper or yelp is used only when the dog intends to communicate its distress to a pack member (or human) to whom they are submissive or friendly, and the other dog or human is expected to react positively to the communication; dogs engaged in serious fights do not whimper lest they betray weakness. Dogs also whimper when they are physically abused or neglected by people. Yelps are often associated with the lowering of the tail between the legs. Yelping can also indicate strong excitement when a dog is lonely and is suddenly met with affection, such as when a dog is left alone in a house during the day and its owner comes through the door late at night. Such yelping is often accompanied by licking, jumping, and barking. Yelping is distinct from barking in that it is softer, higher pitched, and lower volume. Dogs will often feign injury by yelping to gain the upper hand over other puppies during play. Play yelps are often confused for a sign of pain or distress: the dog not running away after the yelp occurs reveals the ruse. 2.9.6 See also • Talking animal 2.9.7 Bibliography • Consciousness Explained by Daniel C. Dennett, 1992, ISBN 978-0-316-18066-5 [1] Virányi, Zs., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Miklósi, Á., Csányi, V. 2004. Dogs respond appropriately to cues of humans' attentional focus. Behavioural Processes, 66: 161-172 [2] Elgier, Angel M., Andriana Jakovcevic, Gabriela Barrera, Alba E. Mustaca, and Mariana Bentosela. “Communication between domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans: Dogs are good learners.” Behavioural Processes 81 (2009): 402-08. Web. 28 June 2014.